Dog behavior

Dog behavior
Dogs roughhousing.

Dog behavior refers to the collection of behaviors by the domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, and is believed to be influenced by genetic, social, situational and environmental causes. The domestic dog is a subspecies of the grey wolf, and shares many of its behavioral characteristics. Although there are important and distinct differences between dogs and wolves, contemporary views of dog behavior are heavily influenced by research on wild wolves.

Contents

Social behavior

The social unit of dogs is the pack. From research on wolf packs that are formed in captivity, the pack has traditionally been thought of as a tightly knit group composed of individuals that have earned a ranking in a linear hierarchy, and within which there is intense loyalty. It is believed that dogs were able to be domesticated by and succeed in contact with human society because of their social nature. According to this traditional belief, dogs generalize their social instincts to include humans, in essence "joining the pack" of their owner/handler. However, much of this traditional view is based on findings from grey wolf packs that are formed of unrelated animals in captivity, and thus may not apply to natural wolf packs, natural dog packs, or dogs incorporated into a human household. Research in packs formed in the wild indicates that wolves form a family group, including a breeding pair and their offspring. In these familial packs, the terms "dominance," and "submission" are less useful than "parent," and "offspring," and bring with them a number of misconceptions. While the majority of research to date indicates that domestic dogs conform to a hierarchy around an Alpha-Beta-Omega structure, domestic dogs, like their wild wolf counterparts, also interact in complex hierarchical ways.

The existence and nature of personality traits in dogs have been studied (15,329 dogs of 164 different breeds) and five consistent and stable "narrow traits" identified, described as playfulness, curiosity/fearlessness, chase-proneness, sociability and aggressiveness. A further higher-order axis for shyness–boldness was also identified.[1][2]

Applicable research on grey wolves

Dr. David Mech of the University of Minnesota, who has studied wolves in their natural habitat, claims that much of what is widely believed about wolf packs is mistaken. From observations of wolf packs on Ellesmere Island over more than a decade,[3] he claims that natural wolf packs are not at all similar to those formed in captivity by unrelated wolves. He attributes many of the misconceptions about wolf packs to generalizations from these unnatural packs in captivity, and equates this to erroneous inferences we might draw from generalizing human behavior from studying refugee camps.

Dr. Mech argues that the natural wolf pack is typically a family, with a breeding pair of adult wolves and their offspring. In such cases, the terms "alpha" and "dominant" are less appropriate than "parent." Of course, the parent wolves are both "alpha" and "dominant" (by definition), but he argues that these terms are misleading because they imply that a pack of wolves typically include multiple families and that the members assume a place in a linear hierarchy. A wolf pack should not be seen as a tribe of individuals who have an established place in a hierarchy until a younger dog usurps the role. Rather, a wolf pack should be seen as a family unit, with young wolves of age dispersing into new territories of their own, to find other wolves and begin their own family units.

Mech also states that dominance is rare in wild wolves, and does not arise from sexual competition. Because young wolves usually disperse before age two, and almost always before age three, there is little sexual tension within a pack. Instead of "dominance" and "submission," he uses the terms "assertiveness" and "passiveness" to reflect the role of the wolf in the pack. Dominant breeding pairs led the pack most of the time (71%), and initiated most new behaviors (70%). Leadership behavior in subordinate pack members tended to be followed by dispersion.[4]

Dominance and submission

Properly socialized dogs can interact with unfamiliar dogs of any size and shape and understand how to communicate.

It is believed by some that dogs establish a dominance hierarchy through aggressive play and roughhousing along a continuum of dominance and submission,[citation needed] although the concept of social hierarchies in dogs is unproven and controversial. It is important for successful socialization that puppies participate with their littermates in learning to relate to other dogs. Dogs learn to successfully relate to other dogs by keeping the peace, rather than by constantly fighting to reestablish this hierarchy.

Dominance behavior

Although dogs are commonly characterized in terms of their dominance (e.g., "Fido is the alpha."), there is some controversy as to whether dominance is a stable personality trait.

In wild wolf packs, displays of dominance have been observed to include "licking up," which involves essentially begging for food; "pinning," in which the dominant dog appears to threaten another, which shows submission by rolling over; "standing over"; territorial marking; and more passive expressions of body language, including holding the tail and ears erect, looking directly at other dogs, circling and sniffing other dogs, growling if the other dog moves.

Submissive displays mirror dominant displays and include adopting a posture that is physically lower than other dogs, such as crouching, rolling over on the back and exposing the abdomen, lowering the tail (sometimes to the point of tucking it between the legs), flattening of the ears, averting the gaze, nervously licking or swallowing, dribbling of urine, and freezing or fleeing when other dogs are encountered.

In wolves, recent research has indicated that dominant behaviors have been misinterpreted as personality traits that determine the inidividual's place in a linear hierarchy in the pack. In contrast, Mech (see recent research, below) argues that packs are family units, and that the "alpha" of a pack does not change through struggles for dominance. Rather, he argues that the family unit serves to raise the young, which then disperse to pair up with other dispersed wolves to form a breeding pair, and a pack of their own. This model undermines the popular conception of dominance in wolf social behavior.

Research on canine familiaris has also questioned whether dominance is a personality trait. Svartberg and colleagues (2002) gathered behavioral data from 15,000 dogs of 164 breeds in attempt to identify major personality traits. In an approach similar to those used in humans, the authors performed a factor analysis of their data, and identified five major traits: "Playfulness," "Curiosity/Fearlessness," "Chase-proneness," "Sociability," "Aggressiveness." A similar analysis by Goddard and Beilharz (1985) revealed two major factors in social behavior: "Confidence," and "Aggression–dominance."

These studies suggest that dominance, per se, may not be a personality trait. Rather, underlying personality traits such as aggressiveness, confidence and curiosity may affect the prevalence of dog behaviors that are viewed as dominant.

Human attachment

A Pomeranian as a recipient of human interaction during Christmas.

Research has shown that there are individual differences in the interactions between dogs and their human masters that have significant effects on dog behavior. For instance, Topal and colleagues (1997) have shown that the type of relationship between dog and master, characterized as either companionship or working relationship, significantly affected the dog's performance on a cognitive problem-solving task. They speculate that companion dogs have a more dependent relationship with their owners, and look to them to solve problems. In contrast, working dogs are more independent.

Behavior when isolated

Dogs value the companionship of the others in their "pack" and are sometimes distressed if they are separated from it. Typical reactions when a dog is separated from the pack are barking, howling, digging, and chewing. These activities may distress humans when they need to leave dogs alone for a period of time. However, this behavior, called separation anxiety, can be overcome with training, or at least decreased to the point where it becomes manageable. If young puppies are habituated to periods alone from an early age, this can normally be prevented entirely. Dogs are also crepuscular, meaning their natural period of peak activity is dawn and dusk,[5] and may be content to rest during the day and night (because of this trait, domestic dogs are more likely to show barking and chewing activity at times when people are leaving to or returning from work.[6]). Some owners struggling to deal with this problem resort to devocalization, a controversial practice considered cruel by animal advocates and outlawed in many countries.

Dog play

From a young age, dogs engage in play with one another. Dog play is made up primarily of mock fights. It is believed that this behavior, which is most common in puppies, is training for important behaviors later in life. Research on puppy play has shown that puppies do not engage equally in both dominant and submissive roles in fights; rather, puppies will tend to start play fights with weaker puppies they believe they can dominate.[7] Additionally, puppies will intervene in play engaged by other pairs. In these situations, the puppies overwhelmingly aid the dominant dog. Puppies do not show reciprocity in interventions, suggesting that they prefer to be dominant in a fight, and are being opportunistic in the short-term. In the long-term, intervention may aid the puppies in learning coordination.

A common behavior among domesticated dogs is chasing their own tails. Researchers are not completely certain why dogs chase their own tail, however some research studies found a link between tail-chasing and high cholesterol. A study found that when dogs experience an increase in activity of hormones tied to the "fight or flight" response, it causes dogs to chase their tails more often.[8]

Classification of dog behaviors[9]
Behavior Type Ears Eyes Mouth & Teeth Body Tail Vocalization
Aggressive Forward or back, close to head. Narrow or staring challengingly. Lips open, drawn back to expose teeth bared in a snarl. Possible jaw snapping. Tense. Upright. Hackles on neck up. Completely dominant position. Straight out from body. Fluffed up. Snarl. Growl. Loud bark.
Alert Perked-up. Turning to catch sounds. Open normally or wide. Mouth closed or slightly open with teeth covered. Normal. Possibly standing on tiptoe. Slightly dominant position. Up. Possibly wagging. None. Low whine or alarm bark.
Anxious Partially back. Slightly narrowed. Mouth closed, or slightly open in a "grin." Tense. Slightly lowered in a submissive position. Partially lowered. Low whine or moaning-type bark.
Before chasing Perked-up, forward-pointing. Wide open. Very alert. Mouth slightly open. Excited panting. Tense. Crouched low in a predatory position. Legs bent, poised to run. Extended straight out from body. None.
Curious, eager, excited Perked-up, forward-pointing. Wide open. Mouth open, teeth covered. Possible panting. Normal stance. Possible wiggling, standing on tiptoe, or pacing. Up. Wagging. Excited short barking, whining.
Dominant Up straight or forward. Wide open, staring. Mouth closed or slightly open. Very tall posture. Hackles may be up. Stiffened and fluffed. Up or straight out from body. Low, assertive growl or grunt.
Fearful Laid back flat and low on head. Narrowed, averted. Possibly rolled back in head, whites showing. Lips drawn back to expose teeth. Tense. Crouched low in submissive position. Shivering, trembling. Possible secretion from anal scent glands. Down between legs. Low, worried yelp, whine, or growl.
Before flight Back. Wide open. Possibly rolled back with whites showing. Slightly opened mouth. Possible drooling. Tense. Shivering. Low, poised to run. Low or between legs. None. Possible yelp or whine.
Friendly Perked-up. Wide open. Alert look. Relaxed, possibly slightly open, "smiling" mouth. Normal posture. Still, or possible wiggling of whole rear end. Up or out from body. Wagging. Whimpering, yapping, or short, high bark.
Guarding Perked-up. Forward. Wide open, alert. Mouth slightly open, teeth bared. Snapping or gnashing of teeth. Tense. Rigid. Hackles up. Standing very tall in an aggressive or dominant stance. Rigid. Held straight out from body. Sometimes fluffed. Loud alert bark. Growl. Snarl.
Happy/Playful Perked-up and forward, or relaxed. Wide open. Sparkly/merry-looking. Mouth relaxed and slightly open, teeth covered. Excited panting. Relaxed, or front end lowered, rear end up in the air, wiggling in a play-bow. Excited bouncing and jumping up and down. Circling around and running forward and back in an invitation to play. Wagging vigorously. Excited barking. Soft play-growling.
Predatory Alert. Held forward or backward to catch sounds. Wide open. Staring, focusing. Mouth closed. Rigid. Low to ground, ready to spring forward. Quietly sniffing the air. Straight and low. None.
Subordinate/Submissive Down, flattened against head. Narrowed to slits or wide open, whites showing. Lips pulled way back from teeth in a "grin." Nuzzling or licking other animal or person on face. Lowered to ground, front paw raised. Lying on back, belly up. Possible urine leaking/dribbling. Possible emptying of anal scent glands. Down, between legs. None, or low, worried whining. Possible yelping/whimpering in fear.

See also

References

  1. ^ Kenth Svartberg, Björn Forkman, K (2002-06-14). "Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)". Applied animal behavior science 79 (2): 133–55. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00121-1. http://www.appliedanimalbehaviour.com/article/S0168-1591(02)00121-1/abstract. Retrieved 2010-06-24. 
  2. ^ Svartberg, K.; Tapper, I.; Temrin, H.; Radesater, T.; Thorman, S. (2004-04-26). "Consistency of personality traits in dogs". Animal Behaviour 69 (2): 283–91. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.04.011. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6W9W-4DPYJ2J-4&_user=10&_coverDate=02%2F01%2F2005&_rdoc=1&_fmt=high&_orig=search&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_searchStrId=1380990863&_rerunOrigin=scholar.google&_acct=C000050221&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=10&md5=0c764b2ec4f26d28ab7524c019c1f295. Retrieved 2010-06-24. 
  3. ^ Mech, L. David (1999). "Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs" (PDF). Canadian Journal of Zoology 77: 1196–1203. http://fwcb.cfans.umn.edu/courses/fw8576/SocialDynMech_1999.pdf. 
  4. ^ Peterson, Rolf O. (2002). Canadian Journal of Zoology. [Full citation needed]
  5. ^ The ecology of stray dogs: a study of free-ranging urban animals By Alan M. Beck p. 12
  6. ^ Dogstardaily.com chapter "Home alone"
  7. ^ C. Ward (2007) Doctoral Dissertation
  8. ^ http://dsc.discovery.com/news/2009/03/24/dog-tail-chasing.html
  9. ^ http://www.pawsacrossamerica.com/interpret.html How to Interpret Your Dog's Body Language, Facial Expressions, and Vocalizations, Accessed 2010-01-17, Bashkim Dibra

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Dog training — is the process of teaching skills or behaviours to a dog. This can include teaching a dog to respond to certain commands, or helping the dog learn coping skills for stressful environments. Dog training often includes operant conditioning,… …   Wikipedia

  • Dog Whisperer with Cesar Millan — Format Reality Starring Cesar Millan Country of origin United States …   Wikipedia

  • Dog aggression — is a term used by dog owners and breeders to describe canine to canine antipathy. Aggression itself is usually defined by canine behaviorists as the intent to do harm . Many dogs show displays of aggression such as barking, growling, or snapping… …   Wikipedia

  • Dog attack — Classification and external resources Dogs typically attack using their teeth[1] ICD 10 …   Wikipedia

  • Dog — For other uses, see Dog (disambiguation). Domestic dog Temporal range: 0.015–0 Ma …   Wikipedia

  • Dog park — The recently renovated Tompkins Square Park dog run was the first in New York City, and it was recently named one of the top five dog parks in the United States by Dog Fancy magazine. This article is about the exercise area for dogs; there is… …   Wikipedia

  • Dog society — form a family group in which individuals display assertiveness or passiveness, rather than dominance and submission. While the majority of research to date indicates that domestic dogs conform to a hierarchy around an Alpha Beta Omega structure,… …   Wikipedia

  • Dog learning — IntroductionIt has been thought for years that Dogs simply learn by doing, but most research now a days focuses on the idea that they do not learn simply in this way. Dogs, as a rule, don t generalize. For instance if you teach a dog to sit in… …   Wikipedia

  • dog — dogless, adj. doglike, adj. /dawg, dog/, n., v., dogged, dogging. n. 1. a domesticated canid, Canis familiaris, bred in many varieties. 2. any carnivore of the dogfamily Canidae, having prominent canine teeth and, in the wild state, a long and… …   Universalium

  • Dog odor — Dogs, as all mammals, have natural odors. Natural dog odor can be unpleasant to dog owners especially when dogs are kept inside the home, as some people are not used to being exposed to the natural odor of a non human species living in close… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”