Imitation

Imitation
A newborn rhesus macaque imitates tongue protrusion.

Imitation is an advanced behavior whereby an individual observes and replicates another's. The word can be applied in many contexts, ranging from animal training to international politics.

Contents

Anthropology and social sciences

In anthropology, diffusion theories explain why cultures imitate the ideas or practices of other cultures. Some theories hold that all cultures imitate ideas from one or a few original cultures, the Adam of the Bible, or several cultures whose influence overlaps geographically. Evolutionary diffusion theory holds that cultures influence one another, but that similar ideas can be developed in isolation.

Scholars[1] as well as popular authors[2] have argued that the role of imitation in humans is unique among animals. Psychologist Kenneth Kaye showed that infants' ability to match the sounds or gestures of an adult depends on an interactive process of turn-taking over many successive trials, in which adults' instinctive behavior plays as great a role as that of the infant.[3] These writers assume that evolution would have selected imitative abilities as fit because those who were good at it had a wider arsenal of learned behavior at their disposal, including tool-making and language.

In the mid-20th century, social scientists began to study how and why people imitate ideas. Everett Rogers pioneered innovation diffusion studies, identifying factors in adoption and profiles of adopters of ideas.

Imitation is a replication of a meme.

Neuroscience

That we are capable of imitating movements, actions, skills, behaviors, gestures, pantomimes, mimics, vocalizations, sounds, speech, etc. and that we have particular "imitation systems" in the brain is old neurological knowledge dating back to Hugo Karl Liepmann. Liepmann's model 1908 "Das hierarchische Modell der Handlungsplanung" (the hierarchical model of action planning) is still valid. On studying the cerebral localization of function, Liepmann postulated that planned or commanded actions were prepared in the parietal lobe of the brain's dominant hemisphere, and also frontally. His most important pioneering work is when extensively studying patients with lesions in these brain areas, he discovered that the patients lost (among other things) the ability to imitate. He was the one who coined the term "apraxia" and differentiated between ideational and ideomotor apraxia. In this basic and wider frame of classical neurological knowledge the discovery of the mirror neurons has to be seen, which was made in monkeys but of course holds for man as well.

FMRI human brain studies revealed a network of regions in the inferior frontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex which are typically activated during imitation tasks.[4] It has been suggested that these regions contain mirror neurons similar to the mirror neurons recorded in the macaque monkey.[5] However, it is not clear if macaques spontaneously imitate each other in the wild.

Neurologist V.S. Ramachandran argues that the evolution of mirror neurons were important in the human acquisition of complex skills such as language and believes the discovery of mirror neurons to be a most important advance in neuroscience.[6] However, little evidence directly supports the theory that mirror neuron activity is involved in cognitive functions such as empathy or learning by imitation.[7]

Evidence is accumulating that bottlenose dolphins employ imitation to learn hunting and other skills from other dolphins.[8][9]

Japanese monkeys have been seen to spontaneously begin washing potatoes after seeing humans washing them.[citation needed] Imitation among non-human species is wholly undisputed in serious academic literature.

Animal behavior

Scientists debate whether animals can truly imitate novel actions or whether imitation is uniquely human.[10] The current controversy is partly definitional. Thorndike uses "learning to do an act from seeing it done."[11] It has two major shortcomings: first, by using "seeing" it restricts imitation to the visual domain and excludes, e.g., vocal imitation and, second, it would also include mechanisms such as priming, contagious behavior and social facilitation,[12] which most scientist distinguish as separate forms of observational learning. Thorpe suggested defining imitation as "the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance, or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency."[13] This definition is favored by many scholars, though questions have been raised how strictly the term "novel" has to be interpreted and how exactly a performed act has to match the demonstration to count as a copy.

In 1952 Hayes & Hayes[14] used the "do-as-I-do" procedure to demonstrate the imitative abilities of their trained chimpanzee "Viki." Their study was repeatedly criticized for its subjective interpretations of their subjects' responses. Replications of this study[15] found much lower matching degrees between subjects and models. However, imitation research focusing on the copying fidelity got new momentum from a study by Voelkl and Huber.[16] They analyzed the motion trajectories of both model and observer monkeys and found a high matching degree in their movement patterns.

Paralleling these studies, comparative psychologists provided tools or apparatuses that could be handled in different ways. Heyes[17][18] and co-workers reported evidence for imitation in rats that pushed a lever in the same direction as their models, though later on they withdrew their claims due to methodological problems in their original setup.[19] By trying to design a testing paradigm that is less arbitrary than pushing a lever to the left or to the right, Custance and co-workers[20] introduced the "artificial fruit" paradigm, where a small object could be opened in different ways to retrieve food placed inside—not unlike a hard-shelled fruit. Using this paradigm, scientists reported evidence for imitation in monkeys[21][22] and apes.[23][24][25] There remains a problem with such tool (or apparatus) use studies: what animals might learn in such studies need not be the actual behavior patterns (i.e., the actions) that were observed. Instead they might learn about some effects in the environment (i.e., how the tool moves, or how the apparatus works).[26] This type of observational learning, which focuses on results, not actions, has been dubbed emulation (see Emulation (observational learning)).

See also

References

  1. ^ George Herbert Mead, Mind, Self, and Society; Kenneth Kaye, The Mental and Social Life of Babies, U. Chicago Press 1982
  2. ^ Susan Blackmore, The Meme Machine, Oxford U. Press 2007; Jared Diamond, The Third Chimpanzee, HarperCollins 1992.
  3. ^ In M. Bullowa, ed. Before Speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, Cambridge U. Press 1979; K. Kaye and J. Marcus, Developmental Psychology, 1981, Vol. 17, pp. 258–65.
  4. ^ Marco Iacoboni, Roger P. Woods, Marcel Brass, Harold Bekkering, John C. Mazziotta, Giacomo Rizzolatti. (1999). Cortical Mechanisms of Human Imitation, Science 286:5449
  5. ^ Rizzolatti, G.; Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27:169–92
  6. ^ V.S. Ramachandran, Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind "the great leap forward" in human evolution. Edge Foundation. Retrieved on 2006-11-16.
  7. ^ Dinstein, I.; Thomas, C.; Behrmann, M.; Heeger, D.J. (2008). "A mirror up to nature". Curr Biol 18 (1): R13–8. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.004. PMC 2517574. PMID 18177704. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2517574. 
  8. ^ Krutzen, M.; Mann, J.; Heithaus, M.R.; Connor, R.C.; Bejder, L.; Sherwin, W.B. (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 (25): 8939–43. doi:10.1073/pnas.0500232102. PMC 1157020. PMID 15947077. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1157020. 
  9. ^ "Wild dolphins teaches others tail walking tricks". WDCS, the Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society. http://www.wdcs.org.au/story_details.php?select=20. 
  10. ^ Zentall, T.R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9, 335–53. Full text
  11. ^ Thorndike, E.L. (1898). Animal intelligence. Psychological Review Monographs 2, No. 8.
  12. ^ Heyes, C.M. & Galef, B.G.J. (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego, Academic Press.
  13. ^ Thorpe, W.H. (1963). Learning and Instinct in Animals. London, Methuen.
  14. ^ Hayes, K.J. & Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 450–9.
  15. ^ Custance, D.-M.; Whiten, A. & Bard, K.A. (1995). Can young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes & Hayes (1952) revisited. Behaviour, 132, 837–59.
  16. ^ Voelkl, B. & Huber, L. (2007): Imitation as faithful copying of a novel technique in marmoset monkeys. PLoS one, 2(7), e611. Full text
  17. ^ Heyes, C.M.; Dawson, G.R. & Nokes, T. (1992). Imitation in rats: initial responding and transfer evidence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45 B, 229–40.
  18. ^ Heyes, C.M. & Dawson, G.R. (1990). A demonstration of observational learning in rats using a bidirectional control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42 B, 59–71.
  19. ^ Heyes, C.M.; Ray, E.D.; Mitchell, C.J. & Nokes, T. (2000). Stimulus Enhancement: Controls for Social Facilitation and Local Enhancement. Learning and Motivation, 31, 83–98.
  20. ^ Custance, D.; Whiten, A. & Fredman, T. (1999). Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 13–23.
  21. ^ Bugnyar, T. & Huber, L. (1997). Push or pull: an experimental study on imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 1997, 817–31.
  22. ^ Voelkl, B. & Huber, L. True imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 60, 195–202.
  23. ^ Whiten, A.; Custance, D.M.; Gomez, J.C.; Teixidor, P. & Bard, K.A. (1996). Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 3–14.
  24. ^ Stoinsky, T.S.; Wrate, J.L.; Ure, N.; Whiten, A. (2001). Imitative Learning by Captive Western Lowland Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in a Simulated Food-Processing Task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 272–81.
  25. ^ Whiten, A.; Horner, V.; Litchfield, C.A. & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Learning and Behavior, 32, 36–52.
  26. ^ Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. (2006). Push or pull: emulation versus imitation in great apes and human children. Ethology, 112, 1159–69. Full text
  • Liepmann, H. (1900). Das Krankheitsbild der Apraxie (motorische Asymbolie). S. Karger Publ., Berlin.
  • Liepmann, H. (1905). Über die Störungen des Handelns bei Gehirnkranken. S. Karger Verlag, Berlin.
  • Liepmann, H. (1908). Drei Aufsätze aus dem Apraxiegebiet. S. Karger Publ., Berlin.
  • Liepmann, H. (1920). Apraxie. Ergebn ges Med 1: 516–43.

Further reading

  • Zentall, T.R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9, 335–53. Full text

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР
Synonyms:

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Imitation — Imitation …   Deutsch Wörterbuch

  • IMITATION — Ouvert par la critique platonicienne de la mimesis (République , Liv. III, 393 398 et X, 595 608), le débat sur l’imitation a constamment été enrichi d’exemples destinés à comparer et à opposer les esprits créateurs, vraiment originaux, d’un côté …   Encyclopédie Universelle

  • imitation — Imitation. s. f. v. L Action, ou l effet de celuy qui imite. Parfaite imitation. heureuse imitation. l imitation de la vie, des vertus, des actions de &c. l imitation d un ouvrage, d un tableau, d un poëme &c. cette piece est une imitation de… …   Dictionnaire de l'Académie française

  • Imitation — Im i*ta tion, n. [L. imitatio: cf. F. imitation.] 1. The act of imitating. [1913 Webster] Poesy is an art of imitation, . . . that is to say, a representing, counterfeiting, or figuring forth. Sir P. Sidney. [1913 Webster] 2. That which is made… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • Imitation — (von lat. imitatio ‚Nachahmung‘ oder ‚Nachbildung‘) steht für Nachahmung einer anderen Person, siehe Imitator Attrappe, eine Nachbildung eines Originals zu dessen Ersatz oder seiner Vortäuschung Fälschung, allgemein das Ausgeben einer Nachahmung… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • imitation — ► NOUN 1) the action of imitating. 2) a copy. ● imitation is the sincerest form of flattery Cf. ↑imitation is the sincerest form of flattery …   English terms dictionary

  • imitation — (n.) c.1400, emulation; act of copying, from O.Fr. imitacion, from L. imitationem (nom. imitatio) a copying, imitation, from pp. stem of imitari to copy, portray, imitate, from PIE *im eto , from root *aim copy. Meaning an artificial likeness is… …   Etymology dictionary

  • imitation — [im΄i tā′shən] n. [L imitatio] 1. the act of imitating 2. a) the result or product of imitating; artificial likeness; copy b) a counterfeit 3. Biol. MIMICRY (sense 2) …   English World dictionary

  • Imitation — (lat.), Nachahmung; auch etwas Nachgeahmtes, besonders auf dem Gebiete der Kunstindustrie, der Juwelierkunst. Vgl. Lehner, Die Imitationen. Anleitung zur Nachahmung von Natur und Kunstprodukten (2. Aufl., Wien 1892). – Über I. in der Musik s.… …   Meyers Großes Konversations-Lexikon

  • Imitation — Imitation, s.v.w. Nachahmung. Im Gebiete des Bauwesens ist die Nachahmung echter und teurer Stoffe durch Malerei oder durch minder kostbare Stoffe ein oft gebrauchtes Mittel, um Wirkungen für den Augenblick oder für vorübergehende Zwecke zu… …   Lexikon der gesamten Technik

  • Imitation — (lat.), Nachahmung; Imitātor, Nachahmer, Nachäffer; imitatōrisch, nachahmend; imitieren, nachahmen …   Kleines Konversations-Lexikon

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”