Cheating (biology)

Cheating (biology)

Cheating is a metaphor commonly used in behavioral ecology to describe organisms that receive a benefit at the cost of others. Cheating is common in many mutualistic and altruistic relationships.[1] Natural selection favors cheating, but there are mechanisms to regulate cheating.[2]

Contents

Theoretical Models

The producer/scrounger game contains cheaters. Producers search for their own food while scroungers take the food others have found. Producers benefit when scroungers are common. Scroungers benefit when scroungers are rare.[3] If there are many scroungers and few producers, the producers are easily able to find food since there is much less competition. If there are few scroungers and many producers, the scroungers can easily cheat. The scroungers are no longer competing against each other to take the producer’s food. A bird can be a scrounger. Instead of foraging for its own food, the scrounger will wait until another bird comes back with food. The next day, the scrounger will follow the successful bird to the patch. The cheating bird reaps the benefits without putting in the effort that the producer put in to find the food.

Animal Examples

Client fish aggregate at cleaning stations, where fish such as cleaner wrasses eat the ectoparasites off the clients. The cleaner wrasse may cheat by eating tissues, scales, and mucus off the client fish instead of ectoparasites.

Some examples of animals that are known to cheat include: cleaner fish, social insects, ungulates, and birds. Cleaner fish have a mutualistic relationship with their clients. The cleaners eat ectoparasites off their clients. The clients benefit from being cleaned, and the cleaners benefit from acquiring food. However, some cleaners cheat by eating tissue, scales, and mucus off their clients instead of only ectoparasites.[4] This could be detrimental to the clients and possibly turn the mutualistic relationship into commensalism or parasitism.[5]
As for social insects, such as bees, ants, and wasps, the queen is the only individual that is supposed to reproduce. In some cases, cheating workers reproduce also.[6] This disrupts the altruistic relationship between the queen and the workers.
Vigilance is essential for many colonial animals. The animals on the outside of the group will be vigilant, while those on the inside forage. The animals within the group also monitor each other’s vigilance.[7] A cheater may emerge in groups with vigilant animals. The cheater might try to forage the entire time, while the rest of the group switches between foraging and being vigilant.

Non-animal Examples

C. tuberosus var. tuberosus is a nectarless orchid that cheats by not supplying nectar to the animals that pollinate it.

The relationships between flowering plants and their animal pollinators and plants and fungi are generally mutualistic, but there are cheaters. Two species of yucca moths eat yucca seeds, but do not pollinate the yucca because they lack the necessary mouthparts.[8] The animal pollinators are not the only cheaters though. Some flowers do not produce nectar. Many pollinators are not able to distinguish between which flowers produce nectar and which do not; therefore, some flowers cheat by not making nectar for the pollinator.[9] Examples of nectarless plants include some species of orchids.[10] Orchids are not just cheaters towards pollinators. Some orchids cheat by not providing fixed carbon to mutualistic fungi.[11]

Solutions

There are ways to avoid cheaters, including: wikt:policing, changing partners, and pushing the cheater on the outside of a vigilant group. In order to avoid cheating in polyandrous honeybees, worker policing is enforced, which entails the removal of eggs laid by selfish workers.[12] To prevent cheating cleaner fish, the clients chase them or switch partners.[13] Furthermore, in order to prevent a cheater emerging in a vigilant group, the group members will force the cheater on the outside, where he/she has to be vigilant. The group members may also physically beat the cheater. If the cheater does not conform, he/she will be kicked out.[citation needed]

References

  1. ^ Ferriere, R., Bronstein, J.L., Rinaldi, S., Law, R., Gauduchon, M. (2002). "Cheating and the evolutionary stability of mutualisms". Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 269 (1493): 773–780. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1900. 
  2. ^ Foster, K. R., Kokko, H. (2006). "Cheating can stabilize cooperation in mutualisms". Proc. R. Soc. B. 273 (1598): 2233–2239. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3571. PMC 1635526. PMID 16901844. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1635526. 
  3. ^ Mathot, K. J., Giraldeau, L. (2008). "Increasing vulnerability to predation increases preference for the scrounger foraging tactic". Behav. Ecol. 19 (1): 131–138. 
  4. ^ Freckleton, R.P., Cote, I.M. (2003). "Honesty and cheating in cleaning symbioses: evolutionary stable strategies defined by variable pay-offs". Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 270 (1512): 299–305. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2232. 
  5. ^ Cheney, K. L., Cote, I. M. (2005). "Mutualism or parasitism? The variable outcome of cleaning symbioses". Biol. Lett. 1 (2): 162–165. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0288. PMC 1626222. PMID 17148155. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1626222. 
  6. ^ Oxley, P.R., Thompson, G.J., Oldroyd, B.P. (2008). "Four Quantitative Trait Loci That Influence Worker Sterility in the Honeybee (Apis mellifera)". Genetics 179 (3): 1337–1343. doi:10.1534/genetics.108.087270. PMC 2475737. PMID 18562647. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2475737. 
  7. ^ Beauchamp, G. (2009). "Sleeping gulls monitor the vigilance behaviour of their neighbours". Biol. Lett. 5 (1): 9–11. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0490. PMC 2657747. PMID 18940772. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2657747. 
  8. ^ Althoff, D. M., Segraves, K. A., Leebens-Mack, J., Pellmyr, O. (2006). "Patterns of Speciation in the Yucca Moths: Parallel Species Radiations within the Tegeticula yuccasella Species Complex". Syst. Biol. 55 (3): 398–410. doi:10.1080/10635150600697325. PMID 16684719. 
  9. ^ Anand, C., Umranikar, C., Shintre, P., Damle, A., Kale, J., Joshi, J., Watve, M. (2007). "Presence of two types of flowers with respect to nectar sugar in two gregariously flowering species". J. Biosci. 32 (4): 769–774. doi:10.1007/s12038-007-0077-1. PMID 17762150. 
  10. ^ Thakar, J. D., Kunte, K., Chauhan, A. N., Watve, A. V., Watve, M. G. (2003). "Nectarless flowers: ecological correlates and evolutionary stability". Oecologia 136 (4): 565–570. doi:10.1007/s00442-003-1304-6. PMID 12838401. 
  11. ^ Taylor, D. L., Bruns, T. D. (1997). "Independent, specialized invasions of ectomycorrhizal mutualism by two nonphotosynthetic orchids". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 94 (9): 4510–4515. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.9.4510. PMC 20753. PMID 9114020. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=20753. 
  12. ^ Jordan, L. A., Allsopp, M. H., Oldroyd, B. P., Wossler, T. C., Beekman, M. (2008). "Cheating honeybee workers produce royal offspring". Proc. R. Soc. B. 275 (1632): 345–351. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1422. PMC 2593727. PMID 18048282. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2593727. 
  13. ^ Bshary, R., Grutter, A. S. (2005). "Punishment and partner switching cause cooperative behaviour in a cleaning mutualism". Biol. Lett. 1 (4): 396–399. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0344. PMC 1626376. PMID 17148216. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1626376. 

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • International Biology Olympiad — The International Biology Olympiad (IBO) is a science olympiad for high school pupils. The first academic international Olympiads after the (originally Eastern European based) International Mathematical Olympiad were launched under the auspices… …   Wikipedia

  • Mutualism (biology) — Hummingbird Hawkmoth drinking from Dianthus. Pollination is a classic example of mutualism. Mutualism is the way two organisms of different species biologically interact in a relationship in which each individual derives a fitness benefit (i.e.,… …   Wikipedia

  • Competition (biology) — Sea Anemones compete for the territory in tide pools Competition is an interaction between organisms or species, in which the fitness of one is lowered by the presence of another. Limited supply of at least one resource (such as food, water …   Wikipedia

  • Altruism in animals — Altruism is a well documented animal behaviour, which appears most obviously in kin relationships but may also be evident amongst wider social groups, in which an animal sacrifices its own well being for the benefit of another animal. Contents 1… …   Wikipedia

  • Symbiosis — Symbiology redirects here. For use of things that represent other things by association, resemblance, or convention, see Symbology. This article is about the biological phenomenon. For other uses, see Symbiosis (disambiguation). For the Marvel… …   Wikipedia

  • Cheat (disambiguation) — Cheat can refer to: A cheat code, a hidden means of gaining an advantage in a computer or video game Cheat!, a television show on the G4 network The Cheat, a 1915 Cecil B. DeMille movie about a wealthy and domineering Asian gentleman taking… …   Wikipedia

  • Biological interaction — Biological interactions are the effects organisms in a community have on one another. In the natural world no organism exists in absolute isolation, and thus every organism must interact with the environment and other organisms. An organism s… …   Wikipedia

  • Pollination — Carpenter bee with pollen collected from Night blooming cereus …   Wikipedia

  • Reciprocal altruism — Diagram showing reciprocal altruism In evolutionary biology, reciprocal altruism is a behaviour whereby an organism acts in a manner that temporarily reduces its fitness while increasing another organism s fitness, with the expectation that the… …   Wikipedia

  • Signalling theory — Within evolutionary biology, signalling theory refers to a body of theoretical work examining communication between individuals. The central question is when animals with conflicting interests should be expected to communicate honestly .… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”