Synesthesia

Synesthesia
How someone with synesthesia might perceive certain letters and numbers.

Synesthesia (also spelled synæsthesia or synaesthesia, plural synesthesiae or synaesthesiae), from the ancient Greek σύν (syn), "together," and αἴσθησις (aisthēsis), "sensation," is a neurologically based condition in which stimulation of one sensory or cognitive pathway leads to automatic, involuntary experiences in a second sensory or cognitive pathway.[1][2][3][4] People who report such experiences are known as synesthetes.

In one common form of synesthesia, known as grapheme → color synesthesia or color-graphemic synesthesia, letters or numbers are perceived as inherently colored,[5][6] while in ordinal linguistic personification, numbers, days of the week and months of the year evoke personalities.[7][8] In spatial-sequence, or number form synesthesia, numbers, months of the year, and/or days of the week elicit precise locations in space (for example, 1980 may be "farther away" than 1990), or may have a (three-dimensional) view of a year as a map (clockwise or counterclockwise).[9][10][11] Yet another recently identified type, visual motion → sound synesthesia, involves hearing sounds in response to visual motion and flicker.[12] Over 60 types of synesthesia have been reported,[13] but only a fraction have been evaluated by scientific research.[14] Even within one type, synesthetic perceptions vary in intensity[15] and people vary in awareness of their synesthetic perceptions.[16]

While cross-sensory metaphors (e.g., "loud shirt," "bitter wind" or "prickly laugh") are sometimes described as "synesthetic", true neurological synesthesia is involuntary. It is estimated that synesthesia could possibly be as prevalent as 1 in 23 persons across its range of variants.[17] Synesthesia runs strongly in families, but the precise mode of inheritance has yet to be ascertained. Synesthesia is also sometimes reported by individuals under the influence of psychedelic drugs, after a stroke, during a temporal lobe epilepsy seizure, or as a result of blindness or deafness. Synesthesia that arises from such non-genetic events is referred to as "adventitious synesthesia" to distinguish it from the more common congenital forms of synesthesia. Adventitious synesthesia involving drugs or stroke (but not blindness or deafness) apparently only involves sensory linkings such as sound → vision or touch → hearing; there are few, if any, reported cases involving culture-based, learned sets such as graphemes, lexemes, days of the week, or months of the year.

Although synesthesia was the topic of intensive scientific investigation in the late 19th century and early 20th century, it was largely abandoned by scientific research in the mid-20th century, and has only recently been rediscovered by modern researchers.[18] Psychological research has demonstrated that synesthetic experiences can have measurable behavioral consequences, while functional neuroimaging studies have identified differences in patterns of brain activation.[6] Many people with synesthesia use their experiences to aid in their creative process, and many non-synesthetes have attempted to create works of art that may capture what it is like to experience synesthesia. Psychologists and neuroscientists study synesthesia not only for its inherent interest, but also for the insights it may give into cognitive and perceptual processes that occur in synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike.

Contents

Definitional criteria

Although sometimes spoken of as a "neurological condition," synesthesia is not listed in either the DSM-IV or the ICD classifications, since it most often does not interfere with normal daily functioning. It had, however, appeared for many years in both Dorland's and Stedman's medical dictionaries. Indeed, most synesthetes report that their experiences are neutral, or even pleasant.[19] Rather, like color blindness or perfect pitch, synesthesia is a difference in perceptual experience and the term "neurological" simply reflects the brain basis of this perceptual difference (see below for associated cognitive traits).

It was once assumed that synesthetic experiences were entirely different from synesthete to synesthete, but recent research has shown that there are underlying similarities that can be observed when large numbers of synesthetes are examined together. For example, sound-color synesthetes, as a group, tend to see lighter colors for higher sounds[20] and grapheme-color synesthetes, as a group, share significant preferences for the color of each letter (e.g., A tends to be red; O tends to be white or black; S tends to be yellow etc.,[19][21][22]). Nonetheless, there are a great number of types of synesthesia, and within each type, individuals can report differing triggers for their sensations, and differing intensities of experiences. This variety means that defining synesthesia in an individual is difficult, and the majority of synesthetes are completely unaware that their experiences have a name.[19] However, despite the differences between individuals, there are a few common elements that define a true synesthetic experience.

Neurologist Richard Cytowic identifies the following diagnostic criteria of synesthesia:[1][2][3]

  1. Synesthesia is involuntary and automatic.
  2. Synesthetic perceptions are spatially extended, meaning they often have a sense of "location." For example, synesthetes speak of "looking at" or "going to" a particular place to attend to the experience.
  3. Synesthetic percepts are consistent and generic (i.e., simple rather than pictorial).
  4. Synesthesia is highly memorable.
  5. Synesthesia is laden with affect.

Cytowic's early cases included individuals whose synesthesia was frankly projected outside the body (e.g., on a "screen" in front of one's face). Later research showed that such stark externalization occurs in a minority of synesthetes. Refining this concept, Cytowic and Eagleman[3] differentiate between "localizers" and "non-localizers" to distinguish those synesthetes whose perceptions have a definite sense of spatial quality.

Experiences

Synesthetes often report that they were unaware their experiences were unusual until they realized other people did not have them, while others report feeling as if they had been keeping a secret their entire lives, as has been documented in interviews with synesthetes on how they discovered synesthesia in their childhood.[14] The automatic and ineffable nature of a synesthetic experience means that the pairing may not seem out of the ordinary. This involuntary and consistent nature helps define synesthesia as a real experience. Most synesthetes report that their experiences are pleasant or neutral, although, in rare cases, synesthetes report that their experiences can lead to a degree of sensory overload.[19]

Though often stereotyped in the popular media as a medical condition or neurological aberration, many synesthetes themselves do not perceive their synesthetic experiences as a handicap. To the contrary, most report it as a gift—an additional "hidden" sense—something they would not want to miss. Most synesthetes become aware of their "hidden" and different way of perceiving in their childhood. Some have learned how to apply this gift in daily life and work. Synesthetes have used their gift in memorizing names and telephone numbers, mental arithmetic, but also in more complex creative activities like producing visual art, music, and theater.[14]

Despite the commonalities which permit definition of the broad phenomenon of synesthesia, individual experiences vary in numerous ways. This variability was first noticed early on in synesthesia research[23] but has only recently come to be re-appreciated by modern researchers. Some grapheme → color synesthetes report that the colors seem to be "projected" out into the world (called "projectors"), while most report that the colors are experienced in their "mind's eye" (called "associators").[24] It is estimated that approximately one or two per hundred grapheme-color synesthetes are projectors; the rest are associators.[24]

Additionally, some grapheme → color synesthetes report that they experience their colors strongly, and show perceptual enhancement on the perceptual tasks described below, while others (perhaps the majority) do not,[15] perhaps due to differences in the stage at which colors are evoked. Some synesthetes report that vowels are more strongly colored, while for others consonants are more strongly colored.[19] In summary, self reports, autobiographical notes by synesthetes and interviews show a large variety in types of synesthesia, intensity of the synesthetic perceptions, awareness of the difference in perceiving the physical world from other people, the way they creatively use their synesthesia in work and daily life.[14][25] The descriptions below give some examples of synesthetes' experiences, which have been experimentally tested, but do not exhaust their rich variety.

Various forms

Synesthesia can occur between nearly any two senses or perceptual modes, and at least one synesthete, Solomon Shereshevsky, experienced synesthesia that linked all five senses. Given the large number of forms of synesthesia, researchers have adopted a convention of indicating the type of synesthesia by using the following notation x → y, where x is the "inducer" or trigger experience, and y is the "concurrent" or additional experience. For example, perceiving letters and numbers (collectively called graphemes) as colored would be indicated as grapheme → color synesthesia. Similarly, when synesthetes see colors and movement as a result of hearing musical tones, it would be indicated as tone → (color, movement) synesthesia.

While nearly every logically possible combination of experiences can occur, several types are more common than others.

Grapheme → color synesthesia

In one of the most common forms of synesthesia, grapheme → color synesthesia, individual letters of the alphabet and numbers (collectively referred to as graphemes), are "shaded" or "tinged" with a color. While different individuals usually do not report the same colors for all letters and numbers, studies with large numbers of synesthetes find some commonalities across letters (e.g., A is likely to be red).[19][21]

As a child, Pat Duffy told her father, "I realized that to make an R all I had to do was first write a P and draw a line down from its loop. And I was so surprised that I could turn a yellow letter into an orange letter just by adding a line." Another grapheme synesthete says, "When I read, about five words around the exact one I'm reading are in color. It's also the only way I can spell. In elementary school I remember knowing how to spell the word 'priority' [with an "i" rather than an "e"] because ... an 'e' was out of place in that word because 'e's were yellow and didn't fit."[26]

Sound → color synesthesia

According to Richard Cytowic, sound → color synesthesia is "something like fireworks": voice, music, and assorted environmental sounds such as clattering dishes or dog barks trigger color and firework shapes that arise, move around, and then fade when the sound ends.[3] For some, the stimulus type is limited (e.g., music only, or even just a specific musical key); for others, a wide variety of sounds triggers synesthesia.

Sound often changes the perceived hue, brightness, scintillation, and directional movement. Some individuals see music on a "screen" in front of their face. Deni Simon, for whom music produces waving lines "like oscilloscope configurations—lines moving in color, often metallic with height, width and, most importantly, depth. My favorite music has lines that extend horizontally beyond the 'screen' area."[3]

Individuals rarely agree on what color a given sound is (composers Liszt and Rimsky-Korsakov famously disagreed on the colors of music keys); however, synesthetes show the same trends as non-synesthetes do. For example, both groups say that loud tones are brighter than soft tones, and that lower tones are darker than higher tones.

Number form synesthesia

A number form from one of Francis Galton's subjects.[9] Note how the first 12 digits correspond to a clock face.
From Wednesday is Indigo Blue.[3] Note this example's upside-down clock face.

A number form is a mental map of numbers, which automatically and involuntarily appears whenever someone who experiences number-forms thinks of numbers. Number forms were first documented and named by Francis Galton in "The Visions of Sane Persons".[27] Later research has identified them as a type of synesthesia.[10][11] In particular, it has been suggested that number-forms are a result of "cross-activation" between regions of the parietal lobe that are involved in numerical cognition and spatial cognition.[28][29] In addition to its interest as a form of synesthesia, researchers in numerical cognition have begun to explore this form of synesthesia for the insights that it may provide into the neural mechanisms of numerical-spatial associations present unconsciously in everyone.

Personification

Ordinal-linguistic personification (OLP, or personification for short) is a form of synesthesia in which ordered sequences, such as ordinal numbers, days, months and letters are associated with personalities.[7][30] Although this form of synesthesia was documented as early as the 1890s[23][31] modern research has, until recently,[1] paid little attention to this form.

For example, one synesthete says, "T’s are generally crabbed, ungenerous creatures. U is a soulless sort of thing. 4 is honest, but… 3 I cannot trust… 9 is dark, a gentleman, tall and graceful, but politic under his suavity."[31] Likewise, Cytowic's subject MT says, "I [is] a bit of a worrier at times, although easy-going; J [is] male; appearing jocular, but with strength of character; K [is] female; quiet, responsible...."[1]

For some people in addition to numbers and other ordinal sequences, objects are sometimes imbued with a sense of personality. Recent research has begun to show that alphanumeric personification co-varies with other forms of synesthesia, and is consistent and automatic, as required to be considered a form of synesthesia.[7]

Lexical → gustatory synesthesia

In the rare lexical → gustatory synesthesia, individual words and the phonemes of spoken language evoke taste sensations in the mouth. According to James Wannerton, "Whenever I hear, read, or articulate (inner speech) words or word sounds, I experience an immediate and involuntary taste sensation on my tongue. These very specific taste associations never change and have remained the same for as long as I can remember."

Jamie Ward and Julia Simner have extensively studied this form of synesthesia, and have found that the synesthetic associations are constrained by early food experiences.[32][33] For example, James Wannerton has no synesthetic experiences of coffee or curry, even though he consumes them regularly as an adult. Conversely, he tastes certain breakfast cereals and candies that are no longer sold.

Additionally, these early food experiences are often paired with tastes based on the phonemes in the name of the word (e.g., /I/, /n/ and /s/ trigger James Wannerton’s taste of mince) although others have less obvious roots (e.g., /f/ triggers sherbet). To show that phonemes, rather than graphemes are the critical triggers of tastes, Ward and Simner showed that, for James Wannerton, the taste of egg is associated to the phoneme /k/, whether spelled with a "c" (e.g., accept), "k" (e.g., York), "ck" (e.g., chuck) or "x" (e.g., fax). Another source of tastes comes from semantic influences, so that food names tend to taste of the food they match, and the word "blue" tastes "inky."

Research history

The interest in colored hearing dates back to Greek antiquity, when philosophers asked if the color (chroia, what we now call timbre) of music was a quantifiable quality.[34] Isaac Newton proposed that musical tones and color tones shared common frequencies, as did Goethe in his book, "Theory of Color." Despite this idea being false, there is a long history of building color organs such as the clavier à lumières on which to perform colored music in concert halls.[35][35][36]

The first medical description of colored hearing is in a German 1812 thesis.[37] The father of psychophysics, Gustav Fechner reported the first empirical survey of colored letter photisms among 73 synesthetes in 1871,[38][39] followed in the 1880s by Francis Galton.[9][40][41] Research into synesthesia proceeded briskly in several countries, but due to the difficulties in measuring subjective experiences and the rise of behaviorism, which made the study of any subjective experience taboo, synesthesia faded into scientific oblivion between 1930 and 1980.

As the 1980s cognitive revolution began to make inquiry into internal subjective states respectable again, scientists once again looked to synesthesia. Led in the United States by Larry Marks and Richard Cytowic, and later in England by Simon Baron-Cohen and Jeffrey Gray, research explored the reality, consistency, and frequency of synesthetic experiences. In the late 1990s, the focus settled on grapheme → color synesthesia, one of the most common[19][22] and easily studied types. Synesthesia is now the topic of scientific books and papers, Ph.D. theses, documentary films, and even novels.

Since the rise of the Internet in the 1990, synesthetes began contacting one another and creating Web sites devoted to the condition. These early grew into international organizations such as the American Synesthesia Association, the UK Synaesthesia Association, the Belgian Synaesthesia Association, the German Synesthesia Association and the Netherlands Synesthesia Web Community.

Prevalence and genetic basis

Early estimates of prevalence varied widely (from 1 in 20 to 1 in 20,000). These studies all had the methodological shortcoming of relying on self-selection, meaning individuals reporting their experience to investigators. Random population studies later determined that 1 in 23 individuals have some kind of synesthesia, while 1 in 90 have colored graphemes.[17] Colored days of the week and colored graphemes are the most common types.[17][19]

Many studies noted that synesthesia runs in families, consistent with a genetic origin for the condition. Francis Galton's 1880 report noted a familial component. Studies from the 1990s[42][43] that noted a much higher prevalence in women than men (up to 6:1) most likely suffered from a sampling bias due to the fact that women are more likely to self-disclose than men. More recent random samples find an equal sex ratio of 1.1:1.[17]

At first, the observed patterns of inheritance were consistent with an X-linked mode of inheritance because there had been no verified reports of father-to-son transmission, whereas father-to-daughter, mother-to-son and mother-to-daughter transmission were readily observed[1][43][44] However, the first genome-wide association study failed to find X-linkage,[45] and furthermore verified two cases of father-to-son transmission.

Suggestive of incomplete gene penetrance is the situation of identical twins in which only one member of the pair is synesthetic,[46][47] and the observation that synesthesia can skip generations within a family.[48] It is furthermore common for family members to experience different types of synesthesia, suggesting that the gene(s) involved do not lead to invariably specific types of synesthesia.[44] Developmental factors such as gene expression and environment must also play a role in determining which types of synesthesia an individual has (for example, children must interact with culturally learned artifacts such as alphabets and food names).

Objective verification

Reaction times for answers that are congruent with a synesthete’s automatic colors are faster than those whose answer is incongruent.[3]

The simplest approach is test-retest reliability over long periods of time, where synesthetes consistently score much higher—around 90% after years, compared to 30–40% after just a month in non-synesthetes even when they are warned they will be retested—using stimuli of color names, color chips, or a computer-screen color picker providing 16.7 million choices.[1][43]

The automaticity of synesthetic experience. The panel on the left is how a non-synesthete perceives the matrix, while a given synesthete might perceive it like the panel on the right.[28]

Modified versions of the Stroop effect are popular. In the standard paradigm, it is harder to name the ink color of the word "red," for example, when it is printed in blue ink than when the ink is red. Similarly, if a grapheme → color synesthete is shown the digit 4 (which he sees as red, say) in blue ink, he is slower to name the ink color than when it is printed in red. He sees the blue ink, but the same sort of conflict responsible for the standard Stroop effect occurs between the ink color and the automatic synesthetic color of the grapheme. The conflict is strongest when the ink color is the opponent color to the synesthetic one (e.g., red vs. green), indicating that synesthetic color perception uses the same mechanism as the perception of real colors.[49]

Cross-sensory Stroop tests are possible: for example, a music → color synesthete must name a red swatch while listening to a sound that produces a blue sensation,[50] or a musical key → taste synesthete must identify a bitter taste while hearing a musical interval that tastes sweet .[51] Likewise, Stroop tests work even in those for whom merely thinking about a numeral elicits color. Take a person who sees 7 as yellow and 9 as blue, and make the task one of having to say a math solution out loud followed by naming a color square. In the illustration, having to answer “7” and then “yellow” is congruent with the subject’s synesthesia, which unconsciously primes him to respond faster than controls. The automatic blueness of 9, however, interferes with naming the green square, slowing him down compared to controls.

Synesthetic colors can also improve performance for some synesthetes. Inspired by tests for color blindness, Ramachandran and Hubbard presented synesthetes and non-synesthetes with a matrix of 5s in which embedded 2s formed a hidden pattern such as a square, diamond, rectangle or triangle.[28] For someone who sees 2s as red and 5s as green, for example, synesthetic colors help zero in on the embedded figure. Subsequent careful studies have found substantial variability among synesthetes in their ability to do this.[15][24] It certainly does not happen instantaneously; while synesthesia is evoked very early in perceptual processing, it does not occur prior to attention.[52][53]

Possible neural basis

Regions thought to be cross-activated in grapheme-color synesthesia (green=grapheme recognition area, red=V4 color area).[28]

Dedicated regions of the brain are specialized for given functions. Increased cross-talk between regions specialized for different functions may account for the many types of synesthesia. For example, the additive experience of seeing color when looking at graphemes might be due to cross-activation of the grapheme-recognition area and the color area called V4 (see figure).[28] One line of thinking is that a failure to prune synapses that are normally formed in great excess during the first few years of life may cause such cross-activation.

An alternate possibility is disinhibited feedback, or a reduction in the amount of inhibition along normally existing feedback pathways.[54] Normally, excitation and inhibition are balanced. However, if normal feedback were not inhibited as usual, then signals feeding back from late stages of multi-sensory processing might influence earlier stages such that tones could activate vision. Cytowic & Eagleman find support for the disinhibition idea in the so-called acquired forms[3] of synesthesia that occur in non-synesthetes under certain conditions: Temporal lobe epilepsy, head trauma, stroke, and brain tumors. It can likewise occur during stages of meditation, deep concentration, sensory deprivation, or with use of psychedelics such as LSD or mescaline, certain prescription medications or even, in some cases, marijuana.

Functional neuroimaging studies using PET and fMRI demonstrate significant differences between the brains of synesthetes and non-synesthetes. fMRI shows V4 activation in both word → color and grapheme → color synesthetes.[15][55][56] Diffusion tensor imaging allows visualization of white matter fiber pathways in the intact brain. This method demonstrates increased connectivity in fusiform gyrus, intraparietal sulcus and frontal cortex in grapheme-color synesthetes.[57] The degree of white matter connectivity in the fusiform gyrus correlates with the intensity of the synesthetic experience.

Associated cognitive traits

Little is known about what, if any, cognitive traits might be associated with synesthesia. As early as 1980, Richard Cytowic first noted mild difficulties in left-right confusion, arithmetic, and sense of direction.[1] These observations await large-scale confirmation. What has been confirmed is elevated, sometimes photographic, memory.[58] When asked, "What good is it?" synesthetes say, "It helps me remember." Indeed, it was reading Alexander Luria's 1968 book The Mind of a Mnemonist that alerted Cytowic to the link between synesthesia and elevated memory: Luria's subject had a 5-fold synesthesia that gave him extra hooks on which to hang and remember numerous facts.

Autism and epilepsy occur with synesthesia more often than chance predicts. Daniel Tammet, the savant who set a European record for reciting the digits of pi, has all three conditions indicating that they might share an underlying genetic cause. Synesthesia has so far been linked to a region on chromosome 2 that is associated with autism and epilepsy.[45]

Synesthetes are likely to participate in creative activities.[22][59][60] Individual development of perceptual and cognitive skills, and one's cultural environment likely determine the variety in awareness and practical use of synesthetic skills[16][25] These are major topics of ongoing research.

Links with other areas of study

Researchers study synesthesia not only because it is inherently interesting, but also because studying it can offer insights into other questions, such as how the brain combines information from different sensory modalities, referred to as crossmodal perception and multisensory integration.

Tests like this demonstrate that people do not attach sounds to visual shapes arbitrarily. Which shape would you call "Kiki" and which "Bouba?"

An example of this is the bouba/kiki effect. In an experiment first designed by Wolfgang Köhler, people are asked to choose which of two shapes is named bouba and which kiki. 95% to 98% of people choose kiki for the angular shape and bouba for the rounded one. Individuals on the island of Tenerife showed a similar preference between shapes called takete and maluma. Even 2.5 year-old children (too young to read) show this effect.[61]

Ramachandran and Hubbard suggest the kiki/bouba effect has implications for the evolution of language, because the naming of objects is not completely arbitrary.[28] The rounded shape may intuitively be named bouba because the mouth makes a more rounded shape to produce that sound, while a more taut, angular mouth shape is needed to articulate kiki. The sound of K is also harder and more forceful than that of B. Such "synesthesia-like mappings" suggest that this effect might be the neurological basis for sound symbolism, in which sounds are non-arbitrarily mapped to objects and actions in the world.

Given synesthetes' extraordinary conscious experiences, researchers hope that their study will provide better understanding of consciousness and its neural correlates, meaning what the brain mechanisms that make us conscious might be. In particular, synesthesia might be relevant to the philosophical problem of qualia,[4][28][62] given that synesthetes experience extra qualia (e.g., a colored sound).

Artistic investigations

Vision by Carol Steen; Oil on Paper; 15x12-3/4" 1996. A representation of a synesthetic photism experienced during acupuncture.

The word "synesthesia" has been used for 300 years to describe very different things, from poetry and metaphor to deliberately contrived mixed-media applications such as son et lumière shows or odorama. It is crucial to separate artists using synesthesia as an intellectual idea—pseudo-synesthetes such as Georgia O'Keeffe who used such titles as "Music-Pink and Blue"—from those who had the genuine perceptual variety, such as Wassily Kandinsky or Olivier Messiaen.

Synesthetic art historically refers to multi-sensory experiments in the genres of visual music, music visualization, audiovisual art, abstract film, and intermedia.[14][18][63][64][65][66] Distinct from neuroscience, the concept of synesthesia in the arts is regarded as the simultaneous perception of multiple stimuli in one gestalt experience.[67] Only recently can science verify and study synesthesia in artists; for deceased artists, one must interpret (auto)biographical information.

Synesthetic art can refer to either art created by synesthetes or art that attempts to convey the synesthetic experience. It is an attempt to understand the relation between the experiences of born synesthetes, non-synesthetes, and an appreciation of such art by both groups. These distinctions are not mutually exclusive given that art by a synesthete might also evoke synesthesia-like experiences in the viewer.

Contemporary synesthetic artists such as Carol Steen[68] and Marcia Smilack[69] have described in detail how they use their synesthesia to create their artworks. They demonstrate the complex interplay between personal experience and artistic creation.

Synesthesia has been a source of inspiration for artists, composers, poets, novelists, and digital artists. Nabokov writes explicitly about synesthesia in several novels. Kandinsky (a synesthete) and Mondrian (not a synesthete) both experimented with image-music correspondences in their paintings. Scriabin composed color music that was deliberately contrived and based on the circle of fifths, whereas Messiaen invented a new method of composition (the modes of limited transposition) to specifically render his bi-directional sound-color synesthesia. For example, the red rocks of Bryce Canyon are depicted in his symphony Des canyons aux étoiles ("From the Canyons to the Stars"). New art movements such as literary symbolism, non-figurative art, and visual music have profited from experiments with synesthetic perception and contributed to the public awareness of synesthetic and multi-sensory ways of perceiving.[14]

Literary depictions

Synesthesia is sometimes used as a plot device or way of developing a character's inner life. Author and synesthete Pat Duffy describes four ways in which synesthetic characters have been used in modern fiction.

  1. Synesthesia as Romantic ideal: in which the condition illustrates the Romantic ideal of transcending one's experience of the world. Books in this category include The Gift by Vladimir Nabokov.
  2. Synesthesia as pathology: in which the trait is pathological. Books in this category include The Whole World Over by Julia Glass.
  3. Synesthesia as Romantic pathology: in which synesthesia is pathological but also provides an avenue to the Romantic ideal of transcending quotidian experience. Books in this category include Holly Payne’s, The Sound of Blue.
  4. Synesthesia as psychological health and balance: Painting Ruby Tuesday by Jane Yardley, and A Mango-Shaped Space by Wendy Mass.
  5. Synesthesia as Young Adult Literature and Science Fiction: Ultraviolet by R.J. Anderson

Many literary depictions of synesthesia are not accurate. Some say more about an author's interpretation of synesthesia than the phenomenon itself.

People with synesthesia

Determining synesthesia from the historical record is fraught with error unless (auto)biographical sources explicitly give convincing details.

Famous synesthetes include David Hockney, who perceives music as color, shape, and configuration, and who uses these perceptions when painting opera stage sets but not while creating his other artworks.[70] Russian painter Wassily Kandinsky combined four senses: color, hearing, touch, and smell.[3] Vladimir Nabokov describes his grapheme-color synesthesia at length in his autobiography, Speak, Memory and portrays it in some of his characters.[71] Composers include Duke Ellington,[72] Franz Liszt,[73] Nikolai Rimsky-Korsakov,[74] and Olivier Messiaen, whose three types of complex colors are rendered explicitly in musical chord structures that he invented.[3][75] Physicist Richard Feynman describes his colored equations in his autobiography, What Do You Care What Other People Think?[76]

Other notable synesthetes include musicians Billy Joel,[77]p. 89, 91 Itzhak Perlman,[77]p. 53 Ida Maria,[78] Brian Chase[79][80] and Patrick Stump; actress Stephanie Carswell (credited as Stéphanie Montreux); inventor Nikola Tesla; electronic musician Richard D. James aka Aphex Twin (who claims to be inspired by lucid dreams as well as music); and classical pianist Hélène Grimaud. Founder of Pink Floyd, Syd Barrett, is thought to have had synesthesia. Harpist and fiddler Tina Larkin experiences Music/color synesthesia. Although it has not been verified, Pharrell Williams, of the groups The Neptunes and N.E.R.D., claims to experience synesthesia,[81] and to have used it as the basis of the album Seeing Sounds. Singer/songwriter Marina and the Diamonds experiences music → color synesthesia, and reports colored days of the week.[82]

Some artists frequently mentioned as synesthetes did not in fact have the condition. Alexander Scriabin's 1911 Prometheus, for example, is a deliberate contrivance whose color choices are based on the circle of fifths and appear to have been taken from Madame Blavatsky.[3][83] The musical score has a separate staff marked luce whose "notes" are played on a color organ. Technical reviews appear in period volumes of Scientific American.[3] On the other hand, his older colleague Nikolai Rimsky-Korsakov (who was perceived as a fairly conservative composer), was in fact a synesthete.[84]

French poets Arthur Rimbaud and Charles Baudelaire wrote of synesthetic experience but there is no evidence they were synesthetes themselves. Baudelaire's 1857 Correspondances (text available here) introduced the notion that the senses can and should intermingle. Baudelaire participated in a hashish experiment by psychiatrist Jacques-Joseph Moreau, and became interested in how the senses might correspond.[14] Rimbaud later wrote Voyelles (1871) (text available here), which was perhaps more important than Correspondances in popularizing synesthesia, although he later boasted "J'inventais la couleur des voyelles!" [I invented the colors of the vowels!].

Sean Day, synesthete and the President of the American Synesthesia Association, maintains a list of famous synesthetes, pseudosynesthetes, and non-synesthetes who used synesthesia in their art or music.

Synthetic synesthesia

In the late 1970s and early 1980s Steve Mann began to experiment with the use of wearable computing to deliberately create useful forms of synesthesia, by, for example, mapping certain senses to other senses (e.g. sights to sounds or feelings, etc., to help the visually impaired).[85] Additionally, Mann regarded the Internet as what he called a "SixthSense" that can be mapped to the other five senses by way of such synthetic synesthesia (e.g. neckworn instrumentation mapping cyberspace to one or more of the other five senses).[85] He named this system "SyntheticSynesthesia of the SixthSense",[86][87] which is often abbreviated as simply SixthSense.

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f g Cytowic, Richard E. (2002). Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses (2nd edition). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-03296-1. OCLC 49395033. 
  2. ^ a b Cytowic, Richard E. (2003). The Man Who Tasted Shapes. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-53255-7. OCLC 53186027. 
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Cytowic, Richard E; Eagelman, David M (2009). Wednesday is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia (with an afterword by Dmitri Nabokov). Cambridge: MIT Press. pp. 309. ISBN 0-262-01279-9. 
  4. ^ a b Harrison, John E.; Simon Baron-Cohen (1996). Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. ISBN 0-631-19764-8. OCLC 59664610. 
  5. ^ Rich AN, Mattingley JB (January 2002). "Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience perspective". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3 (1): 43–52. doi:10.1038/nrn702. PMID 11823804. 
  6. ^ a b Hubbard EM, Ramachandran VS (November 2005). "Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia". Neuron 48 (3): 509–20. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.012. PMID 16269367. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896-6273(05)00835-4. 
  7. ^ a b c Simner J, Holenstein E (April 2007). "Ordinal linguistic personification as a variant of synesthesia". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19 (4): 694–703. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.694. PMID 17381259. http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.694?url_ver=Z39.88-2003&rfr_id=ori:rid:crossref.org&rfr_dat=cr_pub%3dncbi.nlm.nih.gov. Retrieved 2008-12-27. 
  8. ^ Smilek D, Malcolmson KA, Carriere JS, Eller M, Kwan D, Reynolds M (June 2007). "When "3" is a jerk and "E" is a king: personifying inanimate objects in synesthesia". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19 (6): 981–92. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.981. PMID 17536968. http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.981?url_ver=Z39.88-2003&rfr_id=ori:rid:crossref.org&rfr_dat=cr_pub%3dncbi.nlm.nih.gov. Retrieved 2008-12-27. 
  9. ^ a b c Galton F (1880). "Visualized Numerals". Nature 22 (543): 494–5. doi:10.1038/021494e0. 
  10. ^ a b Seron X, Pesenti M, Noël MP, Deloche G, Cornet JA (August 1992). "Images of numbers, or "When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue"". Cognition 44 (1-2): 159–96. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90053-K. PMID 1511585. 
  11. ^ a b Sagiv N, Simner J, Collins J, Butterworth B, Ward J (August 2006). "What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms?". Cognition 101 (1): 114–28. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.09.004. PMID 16288733. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010-0277(05)00155-1. 
  12. ^ Saenz M, Koch C (August 2008). "The sound of change: visually induced auditory synesthesia". Current Biology 18 (15): R650–R651. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.014. PMID 18682202. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0960-9822(08)00734-3. Retrieved 2008-12-28. 
  13. ^ Day, Sean, Types of synesthesia. (2009) Types of synesthesia. Online: http://home.comcast.net/~sean.day/html/types.htm, accessed 18 February 2009.
  14. ^ a b c d e f g van Campen, Cretien (2007). The Hidden Sense: Synesthesia in Art and Science. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-22081-4. OCLC 80179991. 
  15. ^ a b c d Hubbard EM, Arman AC, Ramachandran VS, Boynton GM (March 2005). "Individual differences among grapheme-color synesthetes: brain-behavior correlations". Neuron 45 (6): 975–85. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.008. PMID 15797557. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896-6273(05)00124-8. 
  16. ^ a b Campen, Cretien van (2009) "The Hidden Sense: On Becoming Aware of Synesthesia" TECCOGS, vol. 1, pp. 1-13.[1]
  17. ^ a b c d Simner J, Mulvenna C, Sagiv N, et al. (2006). "Synaesthesia: the prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences". Perception 35 (8): 1024–33. doi:10.1068/p5469. PMID 17076063. 
  18. ^ a b Campen C (1999). "Artistic and psychological experiments with synesthesia". Leonardo 32 (1): 9–14. doi:10.1162/002409499552948. 
  19. ^ a b c d e f g h Sagiv, Noam; Robertson, Lynn C (2005). Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-516623-X. OCLC 53020292. 
  20. ^ Ward J, Huckstep B, Tsakanikos E (February 2006). "Sound-colour synaesthesia: to what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all?". Cortex 42 (2): 264–80. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70352-6. PMID 16683501. 
  21. ^ a b Simner J, Ward J, Lanz M, et al. (2005). "Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations.". Cognitive Neuropsychology 22 (8): 1069–1085. doi:10.1080/02643290500200122. PMID 21038290. 
  22. ^ a b c Rich AN, Bradshaw JL, Mattingley JB (November 2005). "A systematic, large-scale study of synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations". Cognition 98 (1): 53–84. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.003. PMID 16297676. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010-0277(04)00209-4. 
  23. ^ a b Flournoy, Théodore (2001). Des phénomènes de synopsie (Audition colorée). Adamant Media Corporation. ISBN 0-543-94462-X. 
  24. ^ a b c Dixon MJ, Smilek D, Merikle PM (September 2004). "Not all synaesthetes are created equal: projector versus associator synaesthetes". Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4 (3): 335–43. doi:10.3758/CABN.4.3.335. PMID 15535169. http://openurl.ingenta.com/content/nlm?genre=article&issn=1530-7026&volume=4&issue=3&spage=335&aulast=Dixon. 
  25. ^ a b Dittmar, A. (Ed.) (2007) Synästhesien. Roter Faden durchs Leben? Essen, Verlag Die Blaue Eule.
  26. ^ "Slashdot Discussion". 2006-02-19. http://slashdot.org/comments.pl?sid=140022&cid=11726211. Retrieved 2006-08-14. 
  27. ^ Galton F (1881). "The visions of sane persons." (PDF). Fortnightly Review 29: 729–40. http://www.galton.org/essays/1880-1889/galton-1881-fort-rev-visions-sane-persons.pdf. Retrieved 2008-06-17. 
  28. ^ a b c d e f g Ramachandran VS and Hubbard EM (2001). "Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language." (PDF). Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (12): 3–34. http://psy.ucsd.edu/~edhubbard/papers/JCS.pdf. 
  29. ^ Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (June 2005). "Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6 (6): 435–48. doi:10.1038/nrn1684. PMID 15928716. 
  30. ^ Simner J, Hubbard EM (December 2006). "Variants of synesthesia interact in cognitive tasks: evidence for implicit associations and late connectivity in cross-talk theories". Neuroscience 143 (3): 805–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.018. PMID 16996695. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0306-4522(06)01092-X. 
  31. ^ a b Calkins MW (1893). "A Statistical Study of Pseudo-Chromesthesia and of Mental-Forms". The American Journal of Psychology (University of Illinois Press) 5 (4): 439–64. doi:10.2307/1411912. http://jstor.org/stable/1411912. 
  32. ^ Ward J, Simner J (October 2003). "Lexical-gustatory synaesthesia: linguistic and conceptual factors". Cognition 89 (3): 237–61. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00122-7. PMID 12963263. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027703001227. 
  33. ^ Ward J, Simner J, Auyeung V (2005). "A comparison of lexical-gustatory and grapheme-colour synaesthesia.". Cognitive Neuropsychology 22 (1): 28–41. doi:10.1080/02643290442000022. PMID 21038239. 
  34. ^ Gage, J. Colour and Culture. Practice and Meaning from Antiquity to Abstraction. (London:Thames & Hudson, 1993).
  35. ^ a b Peacock, Kenneth. "Instruments to Perform Color-Music: Two Centuries of Technological Experimentation," Leonardo 21, No. 4 (1988) 397–406.
  36. ^ Jewanski, J. & N. Sidler (Eds.). Farbe - Licht - Musik. Synaesthesie und Farblichtmusik. Bern: Peter Lang, 2006.
  37. ^ Mahling, F. (1926) Das Problem der `audition colorée': Eine historisch-kritische Untersuchung. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 57, 165–301.
  38. ^ Fechner, Th. (1871) Vorschule der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel.
  39. ^ Campen, Cretien van (1996). De verwarring der zintuigen. Artistieke en psychologische experimenten met synesthesie. Psychologie & Maatschappij, vol. 20, nr. 1, pp. 10–26.
  40. ^ Galton F (1880). "Visualized Numerals". Nature 21 (533): 252–6. doi:10.1038/021252a0. 
  41. ^ Galton F. (1883). Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development. Macmillan. http://galton.org/books/human-faculty/. Retrieved 2008-06-17. 
  42. ^ Baron-Cohen S, Harrison J, Goldstein LH, Wyke M (1993). "Coloured speech perception: is synaesthesia what happens when modularity breaks down?". Perception 22 (4): 419–26. doi:10.1068/p220419. PMID 8378132. 
  43. ^ a b c Baron-Cohen S, Burt L, Smith-Laittan F, Harrison J, Bolton P (1996). "Synaesthesia: prevalence and familiality". Perception 25 (9): 1073–9. doi:10.1068/p251073. PMID 8983047. 
  44. ^ a b Ward J, Simner J (2005). "Is synaesthesia an X-linked dominant trait with lethality in males?". Perception 34 (5): 611–23. doi:10.1068/p5250. PMID 15991697. 
  45. ^ a b Asher, JE, Lamb, JA, Brocklebank, D, et al. (2009). "A whole-genome scan and fine-mapping linkage study of auditory-visual synesthesia reveals evidence of linkage to chromosomes 2q24, 5q33, 6p12, and 12p12". The American Journal of Human Genetics 84 (2): 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.01.012. PMC 2668015. PMID 19200526. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2668015. 
  46. ^ Smilek D, Moffatt BA, Pasternak J, White BN, Dixon MJ, Merikle PM (2002). "Synaesthesia: a case study of discordant monozygotic twins". Neurocase 8 (4): 338–42. doi:10.1076/neur.8.3.338.16194. PMID 12221147. 
  47. ^ Smilek D, Dixon MJ, Merikle PM (October 2005). "Synaesthesia: discordant male monozygotic twins". Neurocase 11 (5): 363–70. doi:10.1080/13554790500205413. PMID 16251137. http://www.informaworld.com/openurl?genre=article&doi=10.1080/13554790500205413&magic=pubmed. 
  48. ^ Hubbard EM, Ramachandran VS (2003). "Refining the experimental lever: A reply to Shanon and Pribram" (PDF). Journal of Consciousness Studies 10 (3): 77–84. http://psy.ucsd.edu/~edhubbard/papers/Hubbard_JCS03.pdf. 
  49. ^ Nikolić D, Lichti P, Singer W (June 2007). "Color opponency in synaesthetic experiences". Psychol Sci 18 (6): 481–6. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01925.x. PMID 17576258. http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/openurl?genre=article&sid=nlm:pubmed&issn=0956-7976&date=2007&volume=18&issue=6&spage=481. 
  50. ^ Ward J, Tsakanikos E, Bray A (February 2006). "Synaesthesia for reading and playing musical notes". Neurocase 12 (1): 27–34. doi:10.1080/13554790500473672. PMID 16517513. http://www.informaworld.com/openurl?genre=article&doi=10.1080/13554790500473672&magic=pubmed. 
  51. ^ Beeli G, Esslen M, Jäncke L (March 2005). "Synaesthesia: when coloured sounds taste sweet". Nature 434 (7029): 38. doi:10.1038/434038a. PMID 15744291. 
  52. ^ Edquist J, Rich AN, Brinkman C, Mattingley JB (February 2006). "Do synaesthetic colours act as unique features in visual search?". Cortex 42 (2): 222–31. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70347-2. PMID 16683496. 
  53. ^ Sagiv N, Heer J, Robertson L (February 2006). "Does binding of synesthetic color to the evoking grapheme require attention?". Cortex 42 (2): 232–42. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70348-4. PMID 16683497. 
  54. ^ Grossenbacher PG, Lovelace CT (January 2001). "Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints". Trends Cogn. Sci. 5 (1): 36–41. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01571-0. PMID 11164734. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364-6613(00)01571-0. 
  55. ^ Nunn JA, Gregory LJ, Brammer M, et al. (April 2002). "Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: activation of V4/V8 by spoken words". Nat. Neurosci. 5 (4): 371–5. doi:10.1038/nn818. PMID 11914723. 
  56. ^ Sperling JM, Prvulovic D, Linden DE, Singer W, Stirn A (February 2006). "Neuronal correlates of colour-graphemic synaesthesia: a fMRI study". Cortex 42 (2): 295–303. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70355-1. PMID 16683504. 
  57. ^ Rouw R, Scholte HS (June 2007). "Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia". Nat. Neurosci. 10 (6): 792–7. doi:10.1038/nn1906. PMID 17515901. 
  58. ^ Smilek D, Dixon MJ, Cudahy C, Merikle PM (2002). "Synesthetic color experiences influence memory.". Psychological Science 13 (6): 548–52. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00496. PMID 12430840. 
  59. ^ Domino G (1989). "Synesthesia and Creativity in Fine Arts Students: An Empirical Look.". Creativity Research Journal 2 (1-2): 17–29. doi:10.1080/10400418909534297. 
  60. ^ Dailey A, Martindale C, Borkum J (1997). "Creativity, synesthesia, and physiognomic perception.". Creativity Research Journal 10 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1001_1. 
  61. ^ Maurer D, Pathman T, Mondloch CJ (May 2006). "The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults". Dev Sci 9 (3): 316–22. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x. PMID 16669803. http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/openurl?genre=article&sid=nlm:pubmed&issn=1363-755X&date=2006&volume=9&issue=3&spage=316. 
  62. ^ Gray JA, Chopping S, Nunn J et al. (2002). "Implications of synaesthesia for functionalism: Theory and experiments". Journal of Consciousness 9 (12): 5–31. 
  63. ^ Berman G (1999). "Synesthesia and the Arts". Leonardo 32 (1): 15–22. doi:10.1162/002409499552957. 
  64. ^ Maur, Karin von (1999). The Sound of Painting: Music in Modern Art (Pegasus Library). Munich: Prestel. ISBN 3-7913-2082-3. 
  65. ^ Gage, John D. (1993). Colour and culture: practice and meaning from antiquity to abstraction. London: Thames and Hudson. ISBN 0-500-27818-0. 
  66. ^ Gage, John D. (1999). Color and meaning: art, science, and symbolism. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-22611-9. 
  67. ^ Campen, Cretien van (2009) Visual Music and Musical Paintings. The Quest for Synesthesia in the Arts. In: F. Bacci & D. Melcher. Making Sense of Art, making Art of Sense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  68. ^ Steen, C. (2001). Visions Shared: A Firsthand Look into Synesthesia and Art, Leonardo, Vol. 34, No. 3, Pages 203–208 (doi:10.1162/002409401750286949)
  69. ^ Marcia Smilack Website Accessed 20 Aug 2006.
  70. ^ see Cytowic, Richard E. 2002. Synesthesia: a Union of the Senses. Second edition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, and Cytowic, R.E. & Eagleman, D.M. (2009) Wednesday is Indigo Blue." Cambridge: MIT Press
  71. ^ Nabokov, Vladimir. 1966. Speak, Memory: An Autobiography Revisited. New York: Putnam.
  72. ^ Ellington, as quoted in George, Don. 1981. Sweet man: The real Duke Ellington. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons. Page 226.
  73. ^ Quoted from an anonymous article in the Neuen Berliner Musikzeitung (29 August 1895); quoted in Mahling, Friedrich. 1926. "Das Problem der 'Audition colorée: Eine historische-kritische Untersuchung." Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie; LVII Band. Leipzig: Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft M.B.H. Pp. 165–301. Page 230. Translation by Sean A. Day.
  74. ^ according to the Russian press: Yastrebtsev V. "On N.A.Rimsky-Korsakov's color sound- contemplation." Russkaya muzykalnaya gazeta, 1908, N 39–40, p. 842–845 (in Russian), cited by Bulat Galeyev (1999).
  75. ^ see Samuel, Claude. 1994 (1986). Olivier Messiaen: Music and Color. Conversations with Claude Samuel. Translated by E. Thomas Glasow. Portland, Oregon: Amadeus Press.
  76. ^ Feynman, Richard. 1988. What Do You Care What Other People Think? New York: Norton. P. 59.
  77. ^ a b Seaberg, M. (2011). Tasting the Universe. New Page Books. ISBN 978-1-60163-159-6. 
  78. ^ Cairns, Dan (2008-02-24). "Times Online interview". The Times (London). http://entertainment.timesonline.co.uk/tol/arts_and_entertainment/music/article3404605.ece. Retrieved 2008-07-24. 
  79. ^ Forrest, Emma (March 30, 2009). "Emma Forrest meets New York's favourite art-punk rockers Yeah Yeah Yeahs". guardian.co.uk (London: The Guardian). http://www.guardian.co.uk/music/2009/mar/30/pop-music-yeah-yeah-yeahs. Retrieved 2009-05-07. 
  80. ^ Chase, Brian. "Brian Chase's blog". yeahyeahyeahs.com. http://site.yeahyeahyeahs.com/blog/brian.aspx. Retrieved 2009-05-07. [dead link]
  81. ^ It just always stuck out in my mind, and I could always see it. I don't know if that makes sense, but I could always visualize what I was hearing... Yeah, it was always like weird colors." From a Nightline interview with Pharrell
  82. ^ Loose Women | Marina and the Diamonds - ITV Lifestyle ITV - 27 April 2010 - Retrieved 28 April 2010.
  83. ^ Dann, Kevin T. (1998). Bright colors falsely seen: synaesthesia and the search for transcendental knowledge. New Haven, Conn: Yale University Press. ISBN 0-300-06619-8. 
  84. ^ This is according to an article in the Russian press, Yastrebtsev V. "On N.A.Rimsky-Korsakov's color sound- contemplation." Russkaya muzykalnaya gazeta, 1908, N 39-40, p. 842-845 (in Russian), cited by Bulat Galeyev (1999).
  85. ^ a b Intelligent Image Processing, John Wiley and Sons, 2001
  86. ^ "Cyborg: Digital Destiny and Human Possibility in the Age of the Wearable Computer", Steve Mann with Hal Niedzviecki, ISBN 0385658257 (Hardcover), Random House Inc, 304 pages, 2001.
  87. ^ The Body Electric: An Anatomy of the New Bionic Senses [Hardcover], by James Geary, 2002, 214 pages

Further reading

External links

Scientific resources

Synesthesia associations

On the Web


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно сделать НИР?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • synesthesia — [sin΄əs thē′zhə, sin΄əs thē′zhē ə, sin΄əs thē′zē ə] n. [ModL synaesthesia: see SYN & ESTHESIA] 1. Physiol. sensation felt in one part of the body when another part is stimulated 2. Psychol. a process in which one type of stimulus produces a… …   English World dictionary

  • Synesthesia — A condition in which the normally separate senses are not separate. Sight may mingle with sound, taste with touch, etc. The senses are cross wired. For example, when a digit color synesthete sees or just thinks of a number, the number appears… …   Medical dictionary

  • synesthesia — synesthete /sin euhs theet /, n. synesthetic /sin euhs thet ik/, adj. /sin euhs thee zheuh, zhee euh, zee euh/, n. a sensation produced in one modality when a stimulus is applied to another modality, as when the hearing of a certain sound induces …   Universalium

  • synesthesia —    or synaesthesia, and synesthete or synaesthete    A concomitant sensation; a subjective sensation or image of a sense (as of color) other than the one (as of sound) being stimulated. The American Synesthesia Association (ASA) defines it this… …   Glossary of Art Terms

  • synesthesia — synaesthesia UK [ˌsɪnɪsˈθiːzɪə] / US [ˌsɪnəsθɪʒə] or synesthesia UK / US noun [uncountable] medical a condition in which two or more of the five senses that most people experience separately are mixed so that, for example, a person may see colour …   English dictionary

  • synesthesia — US var. of SYNAESTHESIA. * * * synesthesia var. synæsthesia …   Useful english dictionary

  • Synesthesia in literature — Synesthesia is a neurological condition in which one or more sensory modalities become linked. However, for over a century, the term synesthesia has also been used to refer to artistic and poetic devices which attempt to express a linkage between …   Wikipedia

  • Synesthesia in art — The phrase synesthesia in art has historically referred to a wide variety of artistic experiments in order to synthesize different art disciplines (i.e. music and painting) as can be observed in the genres of visual music, abstract film, computer …   Wikipedia

  • Synesthesia Mandala Drums — The Synesthesia Mandala Drum is an electronic drum pad developed by Vince DeFranco and drummer Danny Carey from Tool.[1] It has 128 strike position detection rings from its center to its edge, along with 127 levels of velocity sensitivity. In its …   Wikipedia

  • Synesthesia (album) — Infobox Album | Name = Synaesthesia (Re release Artwork) Type = Album Artist = Buck 65 Released = 2001 Recorded = ? Genre = Hip hop Length = 45:11 Label = Warner / Endemik Producer = Buck 65 Reviews = Last album = Man Overboard (1999) This album …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”