Crib talk

Crib talk

Crib talk or crib speech is pre-sleep monologue made by young children while in bed. This starts somewhere around one-and-a-half years and usually ends by about two-and-a-half years of age, though children can continue longer.[1][2] It consists of conversational discourse with turn-taking often containing semantically and syntactically coherent question-answer sequences. It may contain word play and bits of song and nursery rhyme.

Crib talk has been found in deaf children in their early sign language.[3] It also occurs in autistic children.[4]

Contents

Types

Crib talk has been divided into three somewhat overlapping varieties.[5]

Enactment sequences

This occurs most commonly in early monologues and is done in a low tone. It concerns using language to bring about action and occurs when playing with toys and dolls as “friends” with language embedded in ongoing play. Whilst like conversational speech, it can occur in long uninterrupted sequences in which the child describes what they are doing.

Proto-narrative

In this a child creates a story about events that have happened or imaginary events in temporal-causal sequences that can be as short as five words or as long as 150. They may include the reciting of stories that have been read to them. They occur throughout the period in which a child engages in crib talk.

Problem focused

These concern what happened in the past, what will happen in the future and how events are organized. They incorporate descriptions used by others to enable prediction.

Function

Such monologues have been argued to play a key role in providing a practice space for developing complex connected discourse,[1] aiding a child to use language as a tool to categorize, explain and know the world,[6] and to “clarify what may originally have been problematic or troublesome”.[7]

Such talk is more complex than that done by children in interactions with others,[1][2][8] and this has been suggested to be due to the freedom to control what they say and so not have their cognitive abilities stretched by having to work out how to respond to what someone has just said.[9]

While similar to private speech which usually starts after 3 and ends about 7,[10] crib talk lacks its self-regulatory instructions.[11] pp. 17–19

Research

Problems

Crib talk is difficult to transcribe because such young children typically have poor pronunciation, and because there may be little context to infer the likely meaning of a child’s words, even with the help of a parent. This has limited the number of children studied and the length of time over which the development of crib talk monologues have been researched.[2]

The children studied are also atypical in that they are the offspring of researchers or their close colleagues, and so are from highly-educated backgrounds. The child studied by Katherine Nelson, for example, was highly precocious in her language abilities, which raises questions about the generality of findings on that one child.[2]

Studies

Crib talk was first studied by Ruth Hirsch Weir on her son Anthony and published in 1962.[1] Two other studies have been carried out by Stan Kuczaj on 14 children between 15–30 months - published in 1983[8] - and Katherine Nelson on Emily in 1989.[11]

Examples

(As spelled in transcripts)

Anthony

Antony is here between 28 and 30 months playing at grammatical transformations and combinations.

Step on the blanket
Where is Anthony’s blanket
Where is Anthony’s blanket (falsetto)
Where’s hiding (falsetto)
Books
Down
Down
Have the books today
I take the white blanket off
On the blanket
Under the blanket
Sleep go
What a blue blanket
What the take the blanket[1]p. 126

Emily

(21 months)

The broke, car broke, the ..
Emmy can’t go in the car.
Go in green car.
No.
Emmy go in the car.
Broken. Broken.
Their car broken, so Mommy Daddy go in their car,
Emmy Daddy go in the car,
Emmy Daddy Mommy go in the car,
broke,
Da … da,
the car … their, their, care broken (continues) .[12]

(23 months)

When mormor get me,
when Mormor mae pretty,
Mommy had a help, my sleep,
Mommy came and Mommy get, get up, time to go home.
When my slep and, and, Mormor came.
Then Mommy coming then get up, time to go ho-o-me.
Time to go home.
Drink p-water [Perrier].
Yesterday did that.
Now Emmy sleeping in regular bed.[13]

In the dark

George Miller has noted that the last two verses of A. A. Milne's poem In the dark in Now We Are Six echoes the verbal play of crib talk.[14]

So — here I am in the dark alone,
    There's nobody here to see;
        I think to myself,
        I play to myself
    And nobody knows what I say to myself;
Here I am in the dark alone,
    What is it going to be?
I can think whatever I like to think,
I can play whatever I like to play,
I can laugh whatever I like to laugh,
    There's nobody here but me.

I'm talking to a rabbit ...
    I'm talking to the sun ...
I think I am a hundred
    I'm one.
I'm lying in a forest ...
    I'm lying in a cave ...
I'm talking to a Dragon ...
    I'm BRAVE.
I'm lying on my left side ...
    I'm lying on my right ...
I'll play a lot tomorrow ...
    ...........
I'll think a lot tomorrow ...
    ...........
I'll laugh ...
                a lot ...
                            tomorrow ...
                            (Heigh-ho !)
                                                    Goodnight

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e Weir RH. (1962). Language in the Crib. University of Michigan; Edition 2, (1970) Mouton. OCLC 300988484
  2. ^ a b c d Nelson K. Oster E., Bruner JS. (Ed). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-02363-5
  3. ^ Petitto LA. (2000). On The Biological Foundations of Human Language. In K. Emmorey and H. Lane (Eds.) The signs of language revisted: An anthology in honor of Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima. Erlbaum
  4. ^ Schaerlaekens A. (1997) Crib speech in autistic and psychotic children: case studies of form, content and function. First Language, 17: 303-319 doi: 10.1177/014272379701705114
  5. ^ Nelson K. (1989). Monologue as representation of real-life experience (pp.27-72) In Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-02363-5
  6. ^ Feldman CF. (1989). Monologue as problem solving narrative (pp. 98-120) In Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-02363-5
  7. ^ Bruner J Lucariello J (1989). Monologue as Narrative Recreation of the World (pp.73-97) In Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-02363-5
  8. ^ a b Kuczaj S. (1983). Crib speech and language play. New York: Springer Verlag. ISBN 978-0-387-90860-1
  9. ^ Bloom L. Margulis C. Tinker E. Fujita N. (1996). Early Conversations and Word Learning: Contributions from Child and Adult. Child Development, 67: 3154-3175. p. 3171 PubMed
  10. ^ Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-72010-6 (Original work published 1934)
  11. ^ a b Nelson K. (1989). Introduction: Monologues in the crib. (pp. 1-24) In Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-02363-5
  12. ^ Nelson K. (1998). Language in cognitive development: the emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge University Press, ISBN 978-0-521-62987-4 . p162
  13. ^ Daiute C. Lightfoot C. (2003). Narrative analysis: studying the development of individuals in society. SAGE. ISBN 978-0-7619-2798-3 p.98
  14. ^ Miller, G. (1962) Foreword by a psychologist, pp. 13-17, In Weir RH. (1962). Language in the Crib. University of Michigan; Edition 2, (1970) Mouton. OCLC 300988484

External links

There are hardly any audio examples posted on the internet (see talk page).


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужна курсовая?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Baby talk — This article is about speech directed at babies. For speech like sounds produced by babies, see babbling. For other uses, see Baby talk (disambiguation). Whoopsie daisy redirects here. For the song by Terri Walker, see L.O.V.E (album). Baby talk …   Wikipedia

  • Now We Are Six — For the album by Steeleye Span, see Now We Are Six (album). 1st edition cover (Methuen) Now We Are Six is a book of thirty five children s verses by A. A. Milne, with illustrations by E. H. Shepard. It was first published in 1927 including poems… …   Wikipedia

  • Speech repetition — Children copy with their own mouths the words spoken by the mouths of those around them. This enables them to learn the pronunciation of words not already in their vocabulary. Speech repetition is the saying by one individual of the spoken… …   Wikipedia

  • List of Rugrats episodes — The following is a list of episodes from the American animated series Rugrats. The show first aired on Sunday, August 11, 1991. The first three seasons aired between 1991 1994. The series returned with two Jewish holiday specials in May 1995 and… …   Wikipedia

  • GEICO Cavemen — The GEICO Cavemen are characters and are trademarks of GEICO in a series of well received television advertisements for the auto insurance company GEICO, that have aired from 2004 to present. The campaign was created by Joe Lawson of The Martin… …   Wikipedia

  • Normal, Illinois — Uptown Normal looking east on North Street …   Wikipedia

  • List of words having different meanings in British and American English: A–L — Differences between American and British English American English …   Wikipedia

  • List of Toy Story characters — This is a list of characters from the Toy Story trilogy which consists of the animated films Toy Story, Toy Story 2, and Toy Story 3. Contents 1 Andy s toys 1.1 Woody 1.2 Buzz Lightyear …   Wikipedia

  • Christmas worldwide — Christmas around the world redirects here. For other uses, see Christmas Around the World (Bradley Joseph album). The Christmas season is celebrated in different ways around the world, varying by country and region. Elements common to many areas… …   Wikipedia

  • Thimble Islands — The Thimbles …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”