Sleep deprivation

Sleep deprivation
Sleep deprivation
Classification and external resources
eMedicine topic list

Sleep deprivation is the condition of not having enough sleep; it can be either chronic or acute. A chronic sleep-restricted state can cause fatigue, daytime sleepiness, clumsiness and weight loss or weight gain.[1] It adversely affects the brain and cognitive function.[2] Few studies have compared the effects of acute total sleep deprivation and chronic partial sleep restriction.[2] Complete absence of sleep over long periods is impossible for humans to achieve (unless they suffer from fatal familial insomnia); brief microsleeps cannot be avoided.[3] Long-term total sleep deprivation has caused death in lab animals.[4]

Contents

Physiological effects

Main health effects of sleep deprivation.[5]
Minor dark circles, in addition to a hint of eye bags, a combination suggestive of minor sleep deprivation.

Generally, sleep deprivation may result in:[6][7]

Diabetes

In 2005, a study of over 1400 participants showed that participants who habitually slept few hours were more likely to have associations with diabetes type 2.[17] However, because this study was merely correlational, the direction of cause and effect between little sleep and diabetes is uncertain. The authors point to an earlier study which showed that experimental rather than habitual restriction of sleep resulted in impaired glucose tolerance (IGT).[18]

Effects on the brain

Sleep deprivation can adversely affect the brain and cognitive function.[19] A 2000 study, by the UCSD School of Medicine and the Veterans Affairs Healthcare System in San Diego, used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technology to monitor activity in the brains of sleep-deprived subjects performing simple verbal learning tasks.[20] The study showed that regions of the brain's prefrontal cortex displayed more activity in sleepier subjects. Depending on the task at hand, the brain would sometimes attempt to compensate for the adverse effects caused by sleep deprivation.

The temporal lobe, which is a brain region involved in language processing, was activated during verbal learning in rested subjects but not in sleep-deprived subjects. The parietal lobe, not activated in rested subjects during the verbal exercise, was more active when the subjects were deprived of sleep. Although memory performance was less efficient with sleep deprivation, greater activity in the parietal region was associated with better short term memory.[21]

A 2001 study at Chicago Medical Institute suggested that sleep deprivation may be linked to serious diseases, such as heart disease and mental illnesses including psychosis and bipolar disorder.[22] The link between sleep deprivation and psychosis was further documented in 2007 through a study at Harvard Medical School and the University of California at Berkeley. The study revealed, using MRI scans, that sleep deprivation causes the brain to become incapable of putting an emotional event into the proper perspective and incapable of making a controlled, suitable response to the event. Sleep deprivation may have been the underlying cause of the overdose deaths of celebrities Heath Ledger, and Anna Nicole Smith.[23]

A study tested 17 right-handed civilian males, between the ages of 21–29 years (mean 24.7 ± 2.8 years), with no history of medical, neurological, psychiatric, or sleep disorder conditions. Their histories also included 7–8 h of nightly sleep on a regular basis, no nicotine use, and low caffeine use (less than 100 mg/day). The negative effects of sleep deprivation on alertness and cognitive performance suggest decreases in brain activity and function, primarily in the thalamus, structure involved in alertness and attention, and in the prefrontal cortex, a region sub-serving alertness, attention, and higher-order cognitive processes.[24]

This study used a combination of positron emission tomography (PET) and Fluorine-2-deoxyglucose (FDG), a marker for regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose (CMRglu) and neuronal synaptic activity.A time series design was used, with progressive sleep deprivation as the independent variable. Repeated measures of absolute regional CMRglu, cognitive performance, alertness, mood, and subjective experiences were collected after 0, 24, 48, and 72 h of sleep deprivation. Additional measures of alertness, cognitive performance, and mood were collected at fixed intervals throughout the sleep deprivation period. These measures were included to place the performance results associated with the PET scans in the context of the circadian rhythm of cognitive performance, as well as to impose a moderate-to-heavy near continuous workload on the subjects as might be anticipated in a real-world sustained operation.[24]

A noted 2002 University of California animal study indicated that non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM) is necessary for turning off neurotransmitters and allowing their receptors to "rest" and regain sensitivity which allows monoamines (norepinephrine, serotonin and histamine) to be effective at naturally-produced levels. This leads to improved regulation of mood and increased learning ability. The study also found that rapid eye movement sleep (REM) deprivation may alleviate clinical depression because it mimics selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). This is because the natural decrease in monoamines during REM is not allowed to occur, which causes the concentration of neurotransmitters in the brain, that are depleted in clinically depressed persons, to increase. Sleep outside of the REM phase may allow enzymes to repair brain cell damage caused by free radicals. High metabolic activity while awake damages the enzymes themselves preventing efficient repair. This study observed the first evidence of brain damage in rats as a direct result of sleep deprivation.[25]

Animal studies suggest that sleep deprivation increases stress hormones, which may reduce new cell production in adult brains.[26]

Effects on growth

A 1999 study[27] found that sleep deprivation resulted in reduced cortisol secretion the next day, driven by increased subsequent slow-wave sleep. Sleep deprivation was found to enhance activity on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (which controls reactions to stress and regulates body functions such as digestion, the immune system, mood, sex, or energy usage) while suppressing growth hormones. The results supported previous studies, which observed adrenal insufficiency in idiopathic hypersomnia.

Effects on the healing process

A study conducted in 2005 showed that a group of rats which were deprived of REM sleep for five days had no significant effect on their ability to heal wounds, compared to a group of rats not deprived of "dream" sleep.[28] The rats were allowed deep (NREM) sleep. However, another study conducted by Gumustekin et al.[29] in 2004 showed sleep deprivation hindering the healing of burns on rats.

Attention and working memory

Among the numerous physical consequences of sleep deprivation, deficits in attention and working memory are perhaps the most important;[2] such lapses in mundane routines can lead to unfortunate results, from forgetting ingredients while cooking to missing a sentence while taking notes. Working memory is tested by such methods as choice-reaction time tasks.[2]

The attentional lapses also extend into more critical domains in which the consequences can be literally life-or-death; car crashes and industrial disasters can result from inattentiveness attributable to sleep deprivation. To empirically measure the magnitude of attention deficits, researchers typically employ the psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) which requires the subject to press a button in response to a light at pseudo-random intervals. Failure to press the button in response to the stimulus (light) is recorded as an error, attributable to the microsleeps that occur as a product of sleep deprivation.

Crucially, individuals' subjective evaluations of their fatigue often do not predict actual performance on the PVT. While totally sleep-deprived individuals are usually aware of the degree of their impairment, lapses from chronic (lesser) sleep deprivation can build up over time so that they are equal in number and severity to the lapses occurring from total (acute) sleep deprivation. Chronically sleep-deprived people, however, continue to rate themselves considerably less impaired than totally sleep-deprived participants.[30] Since people usually evaluate their capability on tasks like driving subjectively, their evaluations may lead them to the false conclusion that they are able to perform tasks that require constant attention when their abilities are in fact impaired.

Impairment of ability

The dangers of sleep deprivation are apparent on the road; the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) reports that one in every five serious motor vehicle injuries is related to driver fatigue, with 80,000 drivers falling asleep behind the wheel every day and 250,000 accidents every year related to sleep,[31] though the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration suggests the figure for traffic accidents may be closer to 100,000.[32] The AASM recommends pulling off the road and taking a 15- or 20-minute nap to alleviate drowsiness.[31]

According to a 2000 study published in the British Medical Journal, researchers in Australia and New Zealand reported that sleep deprivation can have some of the same hazardous effects as being drunk.[33] People who drove after being awake for 17–19 hours performed worse than those with a blood alcohol level of .05 percent, which is the legal limit for drunk driving in most western European countries and Australia. Another study suggested that performance begins to degrade after 16 hours awake, and 21 hours awake was equivalent to a blood alcohol content of .08 percent, which is the blood alcohol limit for drunk driving in Canada, the U.S., and the U.K.[34]

In addition, as a result of continuous muscular activity without proper rest time, effects such as cramping are much more frequent in sleep-deprived individuals. Extreme cases of sleep deprivation have been reported to be associated with hernias, muscle fascia tears, and other such problems commonly associated with physical overexertion.

A 2006 study has shown that while total sleep deprivation for one night caused many errors, the errors were not significant until after the second night of total sleep deprivation.[35] However, combining alcohol with acute sleep deprivation results in a trebled rate of driving off the road when using a simulator.[36]

The National Sleep Foundation identifies several warning signs that a driver is dangerously fatigued, including rolling down the window, turning up the radio, trouble keeping eyes open, head-nodding, drifting out of the lane, and daydreaming. At particular risk are lone drivers between midnight and 6 a.m.[37]

Sleep deprivation can negatively impact performance in professional fields as well, potentially jeopardizing lives. Due largely to the February 2009 crash of a regional jet in Buffalo, NY, which killed 50 people and was partially attributed to pilot fatigue, the FAA is reviewing its procedures to ensure pilots are sufficiently rested.[38] A 2004 study also found medical residents with less than four hours of sleep a night made more than twice as many errors as residents who slept for more than seven hours a night, an especially alarming trend given that less than 11% of surveyed residents were sleeping more than seven hours a night.[39]

Twenty-four hours of continuous sleep deprivation results in the choice of less difficult math tasks without decreases in subjective reports of effort applied to the task. Naturally-caused sleep loss affects the choice of everyday tasks such that low effort tasks are mostly commonly selected. Adolescents who experience less sleep show a decreased willingness to engage in sports activities that require effort through fine motor coordination and attention to details.[40][41]

Great sleep deprivation mimics psychosis: distorted perceptions can lead to inappropriate emotional and behavioral responses.[42]

Microsleeps

Microsleeps occur when a person has a significant sleep deprivation. The brain automatically shuts down, falling into a sleep state for a period that can last from a second to half a minute. The person falls asleep no matter what activity he or she is engaged in. Microsleeps are similar to blackouts and a person experiencing them is not consciously aware that they are occurring.

An even lighter type of sleep has been seen in rats that have been kept awake for long. Local regions went into periods of short (~80 ms) but frequent (~40/min) NREM-like state. Despite the on and off periods where neurons shut off, the rats appeared awake, although they performed worse at tests.[43]

Weight gain/loss

In rats, prolonged, complete sleep deprivation increased both food intake and energy expenditure with a net effect of weight loss and ultimately death.[44] This study hypothesizes that the moderate chronic sleep debt associated with habitual short sleep is associated with increased appetite and energy expenditure with the equation tipped towards food intake rather than expenditure in societies where high-calorie food is freely available.[1]

Several large studies using nationally representative samples suggest that the obesity problem in the United States might have as one of its causes a corresponding decrease in the average number of hours that people are sleeping.[45][46][47] The findings suggest that this might be happening because sleep deprivation could be disrupting hormones that regulate glucose metabolism and appetite.[48]

The association between sleep deprivation and obesity appears to be strongest in young and middle-age adults. Other scientists hold that the physical discomfort of obesity and related problems, such as sleep apnea, reduce an individual's chances of getting a good night's sleep.

Sleep loss is currently proposed to disturb endocrine regulation of energy homeostasis leading to weight gain and obesity. A reduction of sleep duration to 4 h for two consecutive nights has recently been shown to decrease circulating leptin levels and to increase ghrelin levels, as well as self-reported hunger. Similar endocrine alterations have been shown to occur even after a single night of sleep restriction.

In a balanced order, nine healthy normal-weight men spent three nights in a sleep laboratory separated by at least 2 weeks: one night with a total sleep time of 7 h, one night with a total sleep time of 4.5 h, and one night with total sleep deprivation (SD). On a standard symptom-rating scale, subjects rated markedly stronger feelings of hunger after total SD than after 7 h sleep (3.9 +/- 0.7 versus 1.7 +/- 0.3; P = 0.020) or 4.5 h sleep (2.2 +/- 0.5; P = 0.041). Plasma ghrelin levels were 22 +/- 10% higher after total SD than after 7 h sleep (0.85 +/- 0.06 versus 0.72 +/- 0.04 ng mL(-1); P = 0.048) with intermediate levels of the hormone after 4.5 h sleep (0.77 +/- 0.04 ng mL(-1)). Feelings of hunger as well as plasma ghrelin levels are already elevated after one night of SD, whereas morning serum leptin concentrations remain unaffected. Thus, the results provide further evidence for a disturbing influence of sleep loss on endocrine regulation of energy homeostasis, which in the long run may result in weight gain and obesity.[49]

Uses

Scientific study of laboratory animals

This rat is being deprived of restful REM sleep by an animal researcher using a single platform ("flower pot") technique. The water is within 1 cm of the small flower pot bottom platform where the rat sits. At the onset of REM sleep, the exhausted rat will either fall into the deep water only to clamber back to its pot to avoid death from drowning, or its nose will become submerged into the water startling it back to an awakened state.

In science, sleep deprivation (of rodents, e.g.) is used in order to study the function(s) of sleep and the biological mechanisms underlying the effects of sleep deprivation.

Some sleep deprivation techniques are as follows:

  • Gentle handling: During the sleep deprivation period, the animal and its polysomnograph record are continuously observed; when the animal displays sleep electrophysiological signals or assumes a sleep posture, it is given objects to play with and activated by acoustic and if necessary tactile stimuli.[50] Although subjective,[51] this technique is used for total sleep deprivation as well as REM or NREM sleep deprivation. This technique often requires polysomnography.
  • Single platform: During the sleep deprivation period, the animal is placed on an inverted flower pot, the bottom diameter of which is small relative to the animal's size (usually 7 cm for adult rats). The pot is placed in a large tub filled with water to within 1 cm of the flower pot bottom. The animal is able to rest on the pot and is even able to get NREM sleep. But at the onset of REM sleep, with its ensuing muscular relaxation, it will either fall into the water and clamber back to its pot or will get its nose wet enough to awaken it. Thus, this technique is only useful for studying REM sleep deprivation. This was one of the first scientific methods developed (see Jouvet, 1964[52] for cats[53] and for rodents).
  • Multiple platform: In an effort to reduce the elevated stress response induced by the single platform method,[54] researchers developed the "multiple platform" technique of REM sleep deprivation. In this configuration, the animal is placed within a large tank containing multiple platforms, thereby eliminating the movement restriction in the earlier setup.
  • Modified multiple platform: Modification of the multiple platform method where several animals together experience sleep deprivation (Nunes and Tufik, 1994).
  • Pendulum: Animals are prevented from entering into REM sleep by allowing them to sleep for only brief periods of time. This is accomplished by an apparatus that moves the animals' cages backwards and forwards in a pendular motion. At the extremes of the motion, the animals experience postural imbalance, forcing them to walk back and forth to retain their balance.[55]

Interrogation

Sleep deprivation can be used as a means of interrogation, which has resulted in court trials over whether or not the technique is a form of torture.[56]

Under one interrogation technique, a subject might be kept awake for several days and when finally allowed to fall asleep, suddenly awakened and questioned. Menachem Begin, the Prime Minister of Israel from 1977–83, described his experience of sleep deprivation as a prisoner of the NKVD in Russia as follows:

In the head of the interrogated prisoner, a haze begins to form. His spirit is wearied to death, his legs are unsteady, and he has one sole desire: to sleep... Anyone who has experienced this desire knows that not even hunger and thirst are comparable with it.[57]

Sleep deprivation was one of the five techniques used by the British government in the 1970s. The European Court of Human Rights ruled that the five techniques "did not occasion suffering of the particular intensity and cruelty implied by the word torture ... [but] amounted to a practice of inhuman and degrading treatment", in breach of the European Convention on Human Rights.[58]

The United States Justice Department released four memos in August 2002 describing interrogation techniques used by the Central Intelligence Agency. They first described 10 techniques used in the interrogations of Abu Zubaydah. Among them included sleep deprivation. Memos from May 2005 introduced four more techniques and confirmed the combination of interrogation methods were not constituted as torture under United States law.[59]

The question of extreme use of sleep deprivation as torture has advocates on both sides of the issue. In 2006, Australian Federal Attorney-General Philip Ruddock argued that sleep deprivation does not constitute torture.[60] Nicole Bieske, a spokeswoman for Amnesty International Australia, has stated, "At the very least, sleep deprivation is cruel, inhumane and degrading. If used for prolonged periods of time it is torture."[61]

Treatment for depression

Recent studies show sleep deprivation has some potential in the treatment of depression. As many as 60% of patients, when sleep-deprived, show immediate recovery, although most relapse the following night. The effect has been shown to link to increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF).[62] It has been shown that chronotype is related to the effect of sleep deprivation on mood in normal people; those with morningness circadian preference show an increase in depression-dejection scores while those with eveningness preference show a significant decrease.[63]

The incidence of relapse can be decreased by combining sleep deprivation with medication.[64] Many tricyclic antidepressants happen to suppress REM sleep, providing additional evidence for a link between mood and sleep.[65] Similarly, tranylcypromine has been shown to completely suppress REM sleep at adequate doses.

Voluntary

Sleep deprivation can sometimes be self-imposed due to a lack of desire to sleep and/or the habitual use of stimulant drugs (i.e Cocaine, Amphetamines, etc.) Recent studies have also suggested that sleep deprivation produces similar effects in the brain to that of an SSRI in persons with depression, thus insuing a clinical, self-imposed remedy[66]. However, most individuals suffering from clinical depression are not aware that lack of sleep is having a direct positive effect on thinking. Sleep deprivation is also self imposed to achieve personal notoriety in the context of record-breaking stunts. Voluntary sleep deprivation is also utilized in the converting from monophasic sleep to polyphasic sleep.

Sleep apnea

Sleep apnea or an obstructive airway disorder reduces the correct amount of airflow. Nasal problems such as a deviated septum will shut down the airway and increase swelling in the mucus lining and nasal turbinates. Corrective surgery (septoplasty) will maximise the airflow and correct the feedback loop to the brain which keeps the sufferer awake so as not to asphyxiate.

Psychostimulant drugs

Psychostimulant drugs, predominantly methamphetamine,[67] are capable of keeping users awake for 3–15 days at a time with repeated dosing.[68]

Mental illness

The specific causal relationships between sleep loss and effects on psychiatric disorders have been most extensively studied in patients with mood disorders. Shifts into mania in bipolar patients are often preceded by periods of insomnia, and sleep deprivation has been shown to induce a manic state in susceptible individuals. Sleep deprivation may represent a final common pathway in the genesis of mania, and sleep loss is both a precipitating and reinforcing factor for the manic state.[69]

School

A National Sleep Foundation survey found that college/university-aged students get an average of 6.7 hours of sleep each night.[70] Sleep deprivation is common in first year college students as they adjust to the stress and social activities of college life. A study performed by the Department of Psychology at the National Chung Cheng University in Taiwan concluded that freshmen received the shortest amount of sleep during the week.[71] In 1997 the University of Minnesota did research that compared students who went to school at 7:15 a.m. and those who went to school at 8:40 a.m. They found that students who went to school at 8:40 got higher grades and more sleep on weekday nights.[32] One in four U.S. high school students admits to falling asleep in class at least once a week.[72] It is known that during human adolescence, circadian rhythms and therefore sleep patterns typically undergo marked changes. Electroencephalogram (EEG) studies indicate a 50% reduction of deep (stage 4) sleep and a 75% reduction in the peak amplitude of delta waves during NREM sleep in adolescence. School schedules are often incompatible with a corresponding delay in sleep offset, leading to a less than optimal amount of sleep for the majority of adolescents.[73]

Counteracting the effects of sleep deprivation

Several strategies are common in attempting to increase alertness and counteract the effects of sleep deprivation. Caffeine is often used over short periods to boost wakefulness when acute sleep deprivation is experienced; however, caffeine is less effective if taken routinely. Other strategies recommended by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine include prophylactic sleep before deprivation, naps, other stimulants, and combinations thereof. However, the only sure and safe way to combat sleep deprivation is to increase nightly sleep time.[74]

Recovery of cognitive function is accomplished more rapidly after acute total sleep deprivation than after chronic partial sleep restriction.[2] Chronic deprivation is the more common in everyday life. Just one night of recovery sleep can reverse adverse effects of total sleep deprivation. Recovery sleep is more efficient than normal sleep with shorter sleep latency and increased amounts of deep and REM sleep.

Longest period without sleep

Randy Gardner holds the scientifically documented record for the longest period of time a human being has intentionally gone without sleep not using stimulants of any kind. Gardner stayed awake for 264 hours (11 days), breaking the previous record of 260 hours held by Tom Rounds of Honolulu.[75] Lt. Cmdr. John J. Ross of the U.S. Navy Medical Neuropsychiatric Research Unit later published an account of this event, which became well-known among sleep-deprivation researchers.[42][76][77][78]

Other sources claim that the Guinness World Records record stands at 449 hours (18 days, 17 hours), held by Maureen Weston, of Peterborough, Cambridgeshire in April, 1977, in a rocking-chair marathon.[79]

Claims of not having slept in years have been made at times, for certain individuals, but either without scientific verification, or contradicted in independent verification:

  • Never scientifically verified: Thai Ngoc, born 1942, claimed in 2006 to have been awake for 33 years or 11,700 nights, according to Vietnamese news organization Thanh Nien. It was said that Ngoc acquired the ability to go without sleep after a bout of fever in 1973, but other reports indicate he stopped sleeping in 1976 with no known trigger. At the time of the Thanh Nien report, Ngoc suffered from no apparent ill effect (other than a minor decline in liver function), was mentally sound and could carry 100 kg of pig feed down a 4 km road[80], but another report indicates that he was healthy before the sleepless episode but that now he was not feeling well because of sleep deprivation.[citation needed]
  • Contradicted by claimant himself: In January 2005, the RIA Novosti published an article about Fyodor Nesterchuk from the Ukrainian town of Kamen-Kashirsky who claimed to have not slept in more than 20 years. Local doctor Fyodor Koshel, chief of the Lutsk city health department, claimed to have examined him extensively and failed to make him sleep. Koshel also said however that Nesterchuck did not suffer any of the normally deleterious effects of sleep deprivation.[81] However, when a reporter from The Guardian followed up on this report, Nesterchuk said he was getting 2–3 hours of sleep per night, and that "[h]e did not appear to notice the marked difference between never getting to sleep once in 240 months, and getting fewer than the recommended number of hours each week."[82]
  • Contradicted in more accurate reporting: Rhett Lamb[83] of St. Petersburg, Florida, was initially reported to not sleep at all, but actually has a rare condition permitting him to sleep only one to two hours per day in the first three years of his life. He has a rare abnormality called an Arnold-Chiari malformation where brain tissue protrudes into the spinal canal; the skull puts pressure on the protruding part of the brain. The boy was operated on at All Children's Hospital in St. Petersburg in May 2008. Two days after surgery he slept through the night.[84][85]
  • Pathological condition: French sleep expert Michel Jouvet and his team reported the case of a patient who was quasi-sleep-deprived for 4 months, as confirmed by repeated polygraphic recordings showing less than 30 min (of stage I sleep) per night, a condition they named "agrypnia". The 27-year-old man was suffering from Morvan's fibrillary chorea, a rare disease that leads to involuntary movements, and in this particular case extreme insomnia. The researchers found that treatment with 5-HTP restored almost normal sleep stages, however some months after this recovery the patient died during a relapse which was unresponsive to 5-HTP. Despite the extreme insomnia, psychological investigation showed no sign of cognitive deficits, except for some hallucinations.[86]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Taheri S, Lin L, Austin D, Young T, Mignot E (December 2004). "Short Sleep Duration Is Associated with Reduced Leptin, Elevated Ghrelin, and Increased Body Mass Index". PLoS Med. 1 (3): e62. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0010062. PMC 535701. PMID 15602591. http://medicine.plosjournals.org/perlserv/?request=get-document&doi=10.1371/journal.pmed.0010062. 
  2. ^ a b c d e Alhola, Paula; Päivi Polo-Kantola (October 2007). "Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance". Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat (Dove Medical Press) 3 (5): 553–567. PMC 2656292. PMID 19300585. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2656292. "Although both conditions [total and partial SD] induce several negative effects including impairments in cognitive performance, the underlying mechanisms seem to be somewhat different." 
  3. ^ Kushida, Clete Anthony (2005). Sleep deprivation. Informa Health Care. pp. 1–2. ISBN 0824759494. http://books.google.com/?id=EaGWMXsR5XYC&pg=PA183&dq=isbn=0824759494. 
  4. ^ Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann B, "Sleep deprivation in the rat by the disk-over-water method" Behavioural Brain Research Volume 69, Issues 1–2, July–August 1995, Pages 55–63 The Function of Sleep doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00020-T
  5. ^ Reference list is found on image page in Commons: Commons:File:Effects of sleep deprivation.svg#References
  6. ^ a b c d e http://www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au/bhcv2/bhcarticles.nsf/pages/Sleep_deprivation?OpenDocument. 
  7. ^ http://www.apa.org/ed/topss/bryanread.html. 
  8. ^ Morin, Charles M. (2003). Insomnia. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publ.. p. 28 death. ISBN 0306477505. 
  9. ^ a b c National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke – Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep
  10. ^ Teachers of Psychology in Secondary Schools
  11. ^ Smith, Andrew P. (1992). Handbook of Human Performance. London: Acad. Press. p. 240. ISBN 0126503524. 
  12. ^ http://www.fi.edu/learn/brain/sleep.html
  13. ^ a b c d http://www.health.harvard.edu/press_releases/sleep_deprivation_problem.htm
  14. ^ http://web.mit.edu/london/www/magnesium.html
  15. ^ http://www.decp.org/pdfs/nystagmus.pdf
  16. ^ Neural Link Between Sleep Loss And Psychiatric Disorders
  17. ^ Gottlieb DJ, Punjabi NM, Newman AB, et al. (April 2005). "Association of sleep time with diabetes mellitus and impaired glucose tolerance". Arch. Intern. Med. 165 (8): 863–7. doi:10.1001/archinte.165.8.863. PMID 15851636. http://archinte.ama-assn.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=15851636. 
  18. ^ Spiegel, K.; R. Leproult, E. Van Cauter (1999-10-23). "Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function". The Lancet 354 (9188): 1435–9. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(99)01376-8. PMID 10543671. 
  19. ^ http://www.fi.edu/brain/sleep.htm. 
  20. ^ http://health.ucsd.edu/news/2000_02_09_Sleep.html. 
  21. ^ "Lack of Sleep Takes Toll on Brain Power". WebMD. http://www.webmd.com/sleep-disorders/news/20000209/lack-of-sleep-takes-toll-on-brain-power. Retrieved 2010-12-14. 
  22. ^ Goes FS, Zandi PP, Miao K, et al. (February 2007). "Mood-incongruent psychotic features in bipolar disorder: familial aggregation and suggestive linkage to 2p11-q14 and 13q21-33". Am J Psychiatry 164 (2): 236–47. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.236. PMID 17267786. http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/full/164/2/236. 
  23. ^ Furek, Maxim (2010). Sleep Deprivation and the Unholy Trinity of Celebrity Death. Counselor Magazine. http://www.counselormagazine.com/columns-mainmenu-55/79-cultural-trends/1088-sleep-deprivation-and-the-unholy-trinity-of-celebrity-death-. Retrieved 2011-01-08 
  24. ^ a b Thomas, M., Sing, H., Belenky, G., Holcomb, H., Mayberg, H., Dannals, R., Wagner JR., H., Thorne, D., Popp, K., Rowland, L., Welsh, A., Balwinski, S. and Redmond, D. (2000), Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. Journal of Sleep Research, 9: 335–352. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00225.x.
  25. ^ Siegel, Jerome M. (November 2003). "Why We Sleep". Scientific American. http://www.semel.ucla.edu/sleepresearch/sciam2003/sciamsleep.pdf. Retrieved 2008-04-03. 
  26. ^ No sleep means no new brain cells
  27. ^ Vgontzas AN, Mastorakos G, Bixler EO, Kales A, Gold PW, Chrousos GP (August 1999). "Sleep deprivation effects on the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and growth axes: potential clinical implications". Clin. Endocrinol. 51 (2): 205–15. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2265.1999.00763.x. PMID 10468992. http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/links/doi/10.1046/j.1365-2265.1999.00763.x. 
  28. ^ Mostaghimi, L.; Obermeyer, WH; Ballamudi, B; Martinez-Gonzalez, D; Benca, RM (September 2005). "Effects of sleep deprivation on wound healing". Journal of Sleep Research 14 (3): 213–9. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00455.x. PMID 16120095. http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/full/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00455.x. 
  29. ^ Gümüştekín K, Seven B, Karabulut N, et al. (November 2004). "Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats". Int. J. Neurosci. 114 (11): 1433–42. doi:10.1080/00207450490509168. PMID 15636354. 
  30. ^ Van Dongen HA et al. (2002). "The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation". Sleep 26 (2): 117–26. PMID 12683469. http://www.aasmnet.org/Resources/FactSheets/DrowsyDriving.pdf. 
  31. ^ a b "Drowsy Driving Fact Sheet". American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2 December 2009. http://www.aasmnet.org/Resources/FactSheets/DrowsyDriving.pdf. 
  32. ^ a b Siri Carpenter (2001). Sleep deprivation may be undermining teen health. 32. http://www.apa.org/monitor/oct01/sleepteen.html. 
  33. ^ Williamson AM, Feyer AM (October 2000). "Moderate sleep deprivation produces impairments in cognitive and motor performance equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication". Occup Environ Med 57 (10): 649–55. doi:10.1136/oem.57.10.649. PMC 1739867. PMID 10984335. http://oem.bmj.com/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=10984335. 
  34. ^ Dawson, Drew and Kathryn Reid (1997). "Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment". Nature 388 (6639): 235. doi:10.1038/40775. PMID 9230429. 
  35. ^ Drummond, SEAN P.A.; Paulus, MP; Tapert, SF (September 2006). "Effects of two nights sleep deprivation and two nights recovery sleep on response inhibition". Journal of Sleep Research 15 (3): 261–5. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00535.x. PMID 16911028. 
  36. ^ Timothy Roehrs et al. (February 1994). "Sleepiness and ethanol effects on simulated driving". Alcoholism:clinical and experimental research 18 (1): 154–158. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1994.tb00896.x. 
  37. ^ "Drowsy Driving:Key Messages and Talking Points". National Sleep Foundation. 2 December 2009. http://www.sleepfoundation.org/sites/default/files/Drowsy%20Driving-Key%20Messages%20and%20Talking%20Points.pdf. 
  38. ^ Dave Michaels (2 December 2009). "FAA Won't Allow Cockpit Naps as Part of Plan to Reduce Pilot Fatigue". http://www.dallasnews.com/sharedcontent/dws/news/washington/dmichaels/stories/120209dnbuspilotfatigue.30ec3d77b.html. 
  39. ^ Baldwinn, DeWitt C. Jr. and Steven R. Daugherty (2004). "Sleep Deprivation and Fatigue in Residency Training: Results of a National Survey of First- and Second-Year Residents". Sleep 27 (2): 217–223. PMID 15124713. 
  40. ^ Engle-Friedman, Mindy; Suzanne Riela, Rama Golan, Ana M. Ventuneac2, Christine M. Davis1, Angela D. Jefferson, Donna Major (June 2003). "The effect of sleep loss on next day effort". Journal of Sleep Research 12 (2): 113–124. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00351.x. PMID 12753348. 
  41. ^ Engle Friedman, Mindy; Palencar, V; Riela, S (2010). "Sleep and effort in adolescent athletes". J Child Health Care 14 (2): 131–41. doi:10.1177/1367493510362129. PMID 20435615. 
  42. ^ a b Coren, Stanley (1 March 1998). "Sleep Deprivation, Psychosis and Mental Efficiency". Psychiatric Times 15 (3). http://www.psychiatrictimes.com/display/article/10168/54471?pageNumber=1. Retrieved 2009-11-25. 
  43. ^ Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Hanlon EC, Nir Y, Cirelli C, Tononi G (2011). "Local sleep in awake rats". Nature 472 (7344): 443–447. doi:10.1038/nature10009. PMC 3085007. PMID 21525926. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=3085007. Lay summary. 
  44. ^ PMID 2928622
  45. ^ Does the lack of sleep make you fat?, Bristol University Press Release, December 7, 2004
  46. ^ Hasler G, Buysse DJ, Klaghofer R, et al. (June 2004). "The association between short sleep duration and obesity in young adults: a 13-year prospective study". Sleep 27 (4): 661–6. PMID 15283000. 
  47. ^ Gangwisch JE, Malaspina D, Boden-Albala B, Heymsfield SB (October 2005). "Inadequate sleep as a risk factor for obesity: analyses of the NHANES I". Sleep 28 (10): 1289–96. PMID 16295214. 
  48. ^ Van Cauter E, Spiegel K (1999). "Sleep as a mediator of the relationship between socioeconomic status and health: a hypothesis". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 896: 254–61. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08120.x. PMID 10681902. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/resolve/openurl?genre=article&sid=nlm:pubmed&issn=0077-8923&date=1999&volume=896&spage=254. 
  49. ^ SCHMID, S. M., HALLSCHMID, M., JAUCH-CHARA, K., BORN, J. and SCHULTES, B. (2008), A single night of sleep deprivation increases ghrelin levels and feelings of hunger in normal-weight healthy men. Journal of Sleep Research, 17: 331–334. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00662.x.
  50. ^ P. Franken, D.J. Dijk, I. Tobler and A.A. Borbely (1991). "Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature". Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 261 (1 Pt 2): R198–R208. PMID 1858947. 
  51. ^ Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann BM, Gilliland MA, Bauer K (1999). "Effects of method, duration, and sleep stage on rebounds from sleep deprivation in the rat". Sleep 22 (1): 11–31. PMID 9989363. 
  52. ^ Jouvet D, Vimont P, Delorme F, Jouvet M (1964). "[Study of selective deprivation of the paradoxal sleep phase in the cat.]" (in French). C. R. Seances Soc. Biol. Fil. 158: 756–9. PMID 14186938. 
  53. ^ Harry B. Cohen and William C. Dement (1965). "Sleep: Changes in Threshold to Electroconvulsive Shock in Rats after Deprivation of "Paradoxical" Phase". Science 150 (3701): 1318–9. doi:10.1126/science.150.3701.1318. PMID 5857002. 
  54. ^ van Hulzen ZJ, Coenen AM (October 1981). "Paradoxical sleep deprivation and locomotor activity in rats". Physiol. Behav. 27 (4): 741–4. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(81)90250-X. PMID 7323178. 
  55. ^ Van Hulzen ZJ, Coenen AM (December 1980). "The pendulum technique for paradoxical sleep deprivation in rats". Physiol. Behav. 25 (6): 807–11. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(80)90298-X. PMID 7220622. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0031-9384(80)90298-X. 
  56. ^ "Binyam Mohamed torture appeal lost by UK government". BBC News. 2009-10-02. http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/uk_news/8507852.stm. 
  57. ^ Begin, Menachem (1979). White nights: the story of a prisoner in Russia. San Francisco: Harper & Row. ISBN 0-06-010289-6. 
  58. ^ Ireland v. the United Kingdom paragraph 102
  59. ^ "Explaining and Authorizing Specific Interrogation Techniques". The New York Times. 2009-04-17. http://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2009/04/17/us/politics/20090417-interrogation-techniques.html. 
  60. ^ Hassan T (2006-10-03). "Sleep deprivation remains red-hot question". PM (abc.net.au). http://www.abc.net.au/pm/content/2006/s1754821.htm. 
  61. ^ "Sleep deprivation is torture: Amnesty". The Sydney Morning Herald. AAP. 2006-10-03. http://www.smh.com.au/news/National/Sleep-deprivation-is-torture-Amnesty/2006/10/03/1159641317450.html. 
  62. ^ Gorgulu Y, Caliyurt O (Sep 2009). "Rapid antidepressant effects of sleep deprivation therapy correlates with serum BDNF changes in major depression". Brain Res Bull 80 (3): 158–62. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.06.016. PMID 19576267. 
  63. ^ Selvi, Yavuz; Mustafa Gulec, Mehmet Yucel Agargun, Lutfullah Besiroglu (17 Aug 2007). "Mood changes after sleep deprivation in morningness–eveningness chronotypes in healthy individuals". Journal of Sleep Research (European Sleep Research Society) 16 (3): 241–4. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00596.x. PMID 17716271. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/118497375/abstract. Retrieved 2009-06-17. 
  64. ^ Wirz-Justice A, Van den Hoofdakker RH (August 1999). "Sleep deprivation in depression: what do we know, where do we go?". Biol. Psychiatry 46 (4): 445–53. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00125-0. PMID 10459393. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0006-3223(99)00125-0. 
  65. ^ Sleep Disorder Treatments
  66. ^ Full text at PMC: 353042 / 353042
    Citation will be completed automatically in a few minutes.Jump the queue or expand by hand
  67. ^ msnbc.com | Amid recession, meth menace evolves
  68. ^ Morris Police Department | Methamphetamines
  69. ^ WebSciences.org | Psychiatric Co-morbidity
  70. ^ http://sleepdisorders.about.com/cs/sleepdeprivation/a/depstudents.htm. 
  71. ^ Tsai LL, Li SP (2004). "Sleep patterns in college students; Gender and grade differences". J Psychosom Res 56 (2): 231–7. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00507-5. PMID 15016583. http://www.websciences.org/cftemplate/NAPS/archives/indiv.cfm?ID=20041266. 
  72. ^ http://abcnews.go.com/US/wireStory?id=1775003. 
  73. ^ Giedd, Jay N., Brain Imaging Unit, Child Psychiatry Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, US Journal of Adolescent Health, Vol 45(4), Oct, 2009. pp. 319–320.
  74. ^ "Sleep Deprivation Fact Sheet". American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2 December 2009. http://www.aasmnet.org/Resources/FactSheets/SleepDeprivation.pdf. 
  75. ^ Coren S (March 1998). "Sleep Deprivation, Psychosis and Mental Efficiency". Psychiatric Time 15 (3). http://www.psychiatrictimes.com/p980301b.html. 
  76. ^ Eleven days awake, Extract from "Elephants on Acid: And Other Bizarre Experiments," by Alex Boese.
  77. ^ Elephants on Acid: And Other Bizarre Experiments, Alex Boese, ISBN 0156031353, Harvest Books, 5 Nov 2007
  78. ^ Neurological Findings After Prolonged Sleep Deprivation, Ross J. (1965), Archives of Neurology 12:399-403.
  79. ^ Thangham CV (October 2007). "What happens when you stay awake for eleven days?". Digital Journal. http://www.digitaljournal.com/article/242559/What_happens_when_you_stay_awake_for_eleven_days_. 
  80. ^ Thao, Vu Phuong. "Vietnam man handles three decades without sleep". Thanh Nien Daily (Vietnam National Youth Federation). Archived from the original on 2008-05-13. http://web.archive.org/web/20080513061843/http://www.thanhniennews.com/features/?catid=10&newsid=12673. Retrieved 2008-05-26. 
  81. ^ Xenophilia – News Archives: Biology (2005)
  82. ^ "Matters of dispute – Sleepless in Ukraine", Shortcuts, The Guardian, 10 Feb 2005 [1] Accessed 11 May 2010
  83. ^ Childs, Dan (March 30, 2009). "The Boy Who Couldn't Sleep". ABC News. http://abcnews.go.com/Health/story?id=7191766&page=4. Retrieved March 31, 2009. 
  84. ^ http://www.wtsp.com/news/local/story.aspx?storyid=80326# Toddler finally gets a good night sleep
  85. ^ http://abcnews.go.com/GMA/OnCall/story?id=6711810&page=1 Mystery of Sleepless Boy Solved: Boy Who Couldn't Sleep Undergoes Risky, Life-Changing Operation
  86. ^ One case of agrypnia (4 months without sleep) in a morvan disease, favourable action of 5-hydroxytryptophane. C. Fischer-Perroudon, J. Mouret et M. Jouvet. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1974, 36: 1–18

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР

Look at other dictionaries:

  • sleep deprivation — noun The condition of forcibly (by someone else, or by a sleep disorder) being kept awake, to the point of noticeably lower alertness. A dangerous amount of sleep deprivation can cause fatal vehicular accidents. Ant: narcolepsy …   Wiktionary

  • sleep deprivation-induced hallucination —    Also known as sleep deprivation hallucination. The term sleep deprivation refers to the deliberate prevention of sleep. Sleep deprivation may be self induced or induced by others, as in interrogation, torture, or sleep deprivation experiments …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • sleep deprivation hallucination —    see sleep deprivation induced hallucination …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • sleep deprivation — noun a form of psychological torture inflicted by depriving the victim of sleep • Hypernyms: ↑torture, ↑torturing …   Useful english dictionary

  • Tony Wright (sleep deprivation) — Tony Wright, author and consciousness researcher from Penzance, Cornwall, holds multiple endurance records.leep deprivation recordHe claimed the world sleep deprivation record in May 2007. [cite web | author =Daily Mail | url… …   Wikipedia

  • Sleep-deprived driving — is the operation of a motor vehicle while being cognitively impaired by a lack of sleep. Sleep deprivation is a major cause of motor vehicle accidents, and it can impair the human brain as much as alcohol can. According to a 1998 survey, 23% of… …   Wikipedia

  • Sleep deprived driving — is the operation of a motor vehicle while being cognitively impaired by a lack of sleep. Sleep deprivation is a major cause of motor vehicle accidents, and it can impair the human brain as much as alcohol can. According to a 1998 survey, 23% of… …   Wikipedia

  • Sleep (non-human) — Sleep in non human animals refers to how the behavioral and physiological state of sleep, mainly characterized by reversible unconsciousness, non responsiveness to external stimuli, and motor passivity, appears in different categories of animals …   Wikipedia

  • Sleep medicine — is a medical subspecialty devoted to the diagnosis and therapy of sleep disturbances and disorders. From the middle of the 20th century, research has provided increasing knowledge and answered many questions about sleep wake functioning. [cite… …   Wikipedia

  • Sleep disorder — Classification and external resources ICD 10 F51, G47 ICD 9 …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”