Merritt Ruhlen

Merritt Ruhlen
Merritt Ruhlen

Born 1944
Citizenship United States of America
Fields Linguistics
Known for Genetic classification of languages
Influences Joseph Greenberg

Merritt Ruhlen (play /ˈmɛrɪt ˈrlən/; born 1944) is an American linguist known for his work on the classification of languages and what this reveals about the origin and evolution of modern humans. Amongst other linguists, Ruhlen's work is recognized as standing outside the mainstream of comparative-historical linguistics. He is the principal advocate and defender of Joseph Greenberg's approach to language classification.

Contents

Biography

Born Frank Merritt Ruhlen, 1944,[1] Ruhlen studied at Rice University, the University of Paris, the University of Illinois and the University of Bucharest. He received his PhD in 1973 from Stanford University with a dissertation on the generative analysis of Romanian morphology. Subsequently, Ruhlen worked for several years as a research assistant on the Stanford Universals Project, directed by Joseph Greenberg and Charles Ferguson.

Since 1994, he has been a lecturer in Anthropological Sciences and Human Biology at Stanford and co-director, along with Murray Gell-Mann and Sergei Starostin, of the Santa Fe Institute Program on the Evolution of Human Languages.[2] Since 2005, Ruhlen has been on the advisory board of the Genographic Project and held appointment as a visiting professor at the City University of Hong Kong. Ruhlen knew and worked with Joseph Greenberg for three-and-a-half decades and became the principal advocate and defender of Greenberg's methods of language classification.[citation needed]

Books

Ruhlen is the author of several books dealing with the languages of the world and their classifications.

  • A Guide to the Languages of the World (1975) provides information on the phonological systems and classifications of 700 languages, prefaced by background information for linguists as well as non-linguists. A greatly expanded version of this work was published in 2005 on the Santa Fe Institute web site.
  • A Guide to the World’s Languages, Volume I: Classification (1987) includes classification of the world’s languages; a history and analysis of the genetic classification of languages; and a defense of the controversial taxonomic work of Joseph Greenberg.
  • The Origin of Language: Tracing the Evolution of the Mother Tongue (1994a)
  • On the Origin of Languages: Studies in Linguistic Taxonomy (1994b). In 1994, Ruhlen published these two books that have similar themes and titles, but are directed at different audiences. The former book, directed at laypersons, includes exercises in which the readers are invited to classify languages themselves using Greenberg's technique, known variously as "mass comparison" and "multilateral comparison". The latter book is aimed at linguists and maintains that some of the assumptions current among historical linguists are incorrect. One of these assumptions is that the only valid criteria for determining a language family are regular sound correspondences and the reconstruction of its protolanguage. According to Ruhlen, these steps can only be carried out after the fact of familyhood has been established by classification.

Research topics

Multidisciplinary approach

Ruhlen has been in the forefront of attempts to coordinate the results of historical linguistics and other human sciences, such as genetics and archaeology.[3][4][5] In this endeavor he has extensively worked with the geneticist Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza for three decades and with the archaeologist Colin Renfrew for two decades.[citation needed]

Taxonomic methods

Most of the criticism directed at Ruhlen centers on his defense of Joseph Greenberg's technique of language classification,[citation needed] called "mass comparison" or "multilateral comparison." It involves comparing selected elements of the morphology and basic vocabulary of the languages being investigated, examining them for similarities in sound and meaning, and formulating a hypothesis of classification based on these. Ruhlen maintains that such classification is the first step in the comparative method and that the other operations of historical linguistics, in particular the formulation of sound correspondences and the reconstruction of a protolanguage, can only be carried out after a hypothesis of classification has been established.

While Hock, for instance,[6][7] claims that only reconstruction proves genetic affinity, and that Indo-European, Uralic, Dravidian, Austronesian, Bantu, and Uto-Aztecan have all been proved by successful reconstructions, Ruhlen disagrees, saying: And yet all of these families were universally accepted as valid families before anyone even thought of trying to reconstruct the protolanguage.[8] As an example, Ruhlen mentions Delbrück (1842–1922), who considered Indo-European to have been proved by the time of Bopp at the beginning of the 19th century; the basis for this proof was the "juxtaposition of words and forms of similar meaning."[9]

Ruhlen believes his classification of the world's languages is supported by population genetics research by the geneticist Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza, who has identified the distribution of certain human genes in populations throughout the world. He has used this evidence to construct phylogenetic trees showing the evolutionary history of these populations.[10][11] Cavalli-Sforza's findings are argued to match up remarkably well with Ruhlen's language classification. Ruhlen's linguist opponents hold that genetic relatedness cannot be used to adduce linguistic relatedness.

This tree has been criticized by some linguists and anthropologists on several grounds: that it makes selective use of languages and populations (omitting the numerous Sino-Tibetan speakers of northern China, for example); that it assumes the truth of such linguistic groups as Austric and Amerind that are controversial; and that several of the population groups listed are defined not by their genes but by their languages, making the correlation irrelevant to a comparison of genetic and linguistic branching and tautological as well.[12][13]

The Amerind macrofamily

The prevailing opinion on the classification of Western Hemisphere languages is that there are many separate language families in the Americas, among which concrete evidence for genetic affinity is lacking.[14] Greenberg published his contrary hypothesis, Amerind language family, in 1987 in one of his major books, Language in the Americas. According to the Amerind hypothesis, all of the languages of North and South America, except for the Na-Dene and Eskimo–Aleut language families, belong to a single macrofamily. One of Greenberg’s most controversial hypotheses, it was updated by Ruhlen in 2007.[15] Ruhlen has published papers presenting research in support of it, e.g., in 1994,[16][17][18][19] 1995,[20][21][22][23] and 2004.[24]

Ruhlen stresses the importance of the three-way i / u / a (i.e. masculine / feminine / neutral) ablaut in such forms as t'ina / t'una / t'ana ("son / daughter / child") as well as of the general American pronominal pattern na / ma (i.e. "I / you"), first noted by Alfredo Trombetti in 1905. Some linguists have attributed this pronoun pattern to other than genetic causes.[25] He refers to the earliest beginnings of the dispute,[8][26] quoting from a personal letter of Edward Sapir to A.L. Kroeber (1918):[27] "Getting down to brass tacks, how in the Hell are you going to explain general American n- 'I' except genetically? It's disturbing, I know, but (more) non-committal conservatism is only dodging, after all, isn't it? Great simplifications are in store for us."

Greenberg and Ruhlen's views on the languages of the Americas have failed to find acceptance among the vast majority of linguists working with these languages.[14]

Kusunda as an Indo-Pacific language

Whitehouse, Ruhlen, and others have concluded[28] that the Kusunda language of Nepal belongs to the tentative Indo-Pacific superfamily[29] rather than belonging to the Tibeto-Burman group or being a language isolate.[30] They adduce:

1. within the personal pronouns,
(a) an independent first-person pronoun based on /t/;
(b) an independent second-person pronoun based on /n/ or /ŋ/;
(c) an independent third-person pronoun based on /g/ or /k/;
(d) a vowel alternation in the first- and second-person independent pronouns in which /u/ occurs in subject forms and /i/ in possessive (or oblique) forms;
2. a possessive suffix -/yi/;
3. the consonantal base also indicates the verbal subject;
4. demonstrative pronouns based on /t/ and /n/;
5. the core vocabulary.

The following table shows similarities between the pronominal systems of several languages claimed to belong to the Indo-Pacific family.[28]

Pronoun Kusunda Andamanese languages Core North
Halmaheran family
Central Bird's
Head family
Juwoi Bo Galela Karon Dori
I chi[31]
tsi[32]
tshi[33]
tui tu-lʌ to tuo
my chí-yi[31] tii-ye ti-e d͡ʒi "me"
you nu[31]
nu[32]
nu[33]
ŋui ŋu-lʌ no nuo
your ní-yí[31] ŋii-ye ni "thee"
he/she gida[31]
git[32]
kitɛ kitɛ gao

The following objections have been made to this tentative proposal:[34]

  1. the existence of an Indo-Pacific superfamily is disputed;
  2. pronouns can be borrowed;
  3. similarities may be due to chance;
  4. linguistic relationships cannot adduced solely on the basis of the physical attributes of the speakers, and the current proposal concurs with an old one allegedly so based;
  5. misrepresentation of the data (e.g., kitɛ in Juwoi is actually a demonstrative meaning "this", never used as a personal pronoun.)

Yeniseian–Na-Dene

According to Ruhlen, linguistic evidence indicates that the Yeniseian languages, spoken in central Siberia, are most closely related to the Na-Dene languages of western North America (among which, concurring with Sapir, he includes Haida).[35] The hypothesis is supported by the separate researches of Heinrich K. Werner[36] and Edward J. Vajda (Vajda rejects Haida's membership in the Na-Dene language family).[37] This would mean that Na-Dene represents a distinct migration of peoples from Asia to the New World, intermediate between the migration of speakers of the putative Proto-Amerind, estimated at around 13,000 years ago, and the migration of Eskimo–Aleut speakers around 5,000 years ago. At other times, Ruhlen has maintained the existence of a language family called Dene–Caucasian.[26][38]

The Proto-Sapiens hypothesis

On the question of the Proto-Sapiens language and global etymologies, most mainstream historical linguists reject Ruhlen's assumptions and methodology,[39][40][41] holding that it is impossible to reconstruct a language spoken at least 30,000 years ago (possibly more than 100,000 years ago). Ruhlen has responded that he (and Bengtson) have never claimed to have reconstructed Proto-Sapiens, but have simply pointed out that reflexes of very ancient words can still be found in the world’s languages:[42] For each [global] etymology ... we present a phonetic and semantic gloss, followed by examples from different language families. ... We do not deal here with reconstruction, and these [semantic and phonetic] glosses are intended merely to characterize the most general meaning and phonological shape of each root. Future work on reconstruction will no doubt discover cases where the most widespread meaning or shape was not original.

Ruhlen also maintains that the “temporal ceiling” assumed by many mainstream linguists – the time depth beyond which the comparative method fails, considered by some[25][43] to lie at roughly 6,000 to 8,000 years ago – does not exist, and that the now universally recognized existence of a language family as old as Afroasiatic, not to mention the even older Eurasiatic (whose existence remains controversial), shows that the comparative method can reach farther into the past than most linguists currently accept.[44]

Notes

  1. ^ Library of Congress Authorities
  2. ^ Starostin 2004
  3. ^ Chen, Sokal, and Ruhlen 1995
  4. ^ Ruhlen 1995e
  5. ^ Knight et al. 2003
  6. ^ Hock 1986
  7. ^ Hock and Joseph 1996
  8. ^ a b Ruhlen 2001d
  9. ^ Delbrück 1880
  10. ^ Cavalli-Sforza et al. 1988
  11. ^ Cavalli-Sforza 2000
  12. ^ Bateman et al. 1990
  13. ^ Trask 1996
  14. ^ a b Campbell 1997
  15. ^ Greenberg and Ruhlen 2007
  16. ^ Ruhlen1994c
  17. ^ Ruhlen 1994d, 177–188
  18. ^ Ruhlen 1994e, 72–73
  19. ^ Ruhlen 1994f
  20. ^ Ruhlen 1995a
  21. ^ Ruhlen 1995b
  22. ^ Ruhlen 1995c
  23. ^ Ruhlen 1995d
  24. ^ Ruhlen 2004
  25. ^ a b Nichols 1992
  26. ^ a b Ruhlen 1994b
  27. ^ Sapir, cited in Sapir 1984
  28. ^ a b Whitehouse et al. 2004
  29. ^ Greenberg 1971
  30. ^ Watters 2006
  31. ^ a b c d e Hodgson 1857
  32. ^ a b c Reinhard 1976
  33. ^ a b Reinhard and Toba 1970
  34. ^ Poser 2004
  35. ^ Ruhlen 1998a
  36. ^ Werner 2004
  37. ^ Vajda 2010
  38. ^ Ruhlen 1998b, 231–246
  39. ^ Kessler 2001
  40. ^ Picard 1998
  41. ^ Salmons 1997
  42. ^ Bengtson and Ruhlen 1994
  43. ^ Kaufman 1990
  44. ^ Ruhlen 1994a, 76–78

Bibliography

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Merritt Ruhlen — (* 10. Mai 1944 in Washington, D.C.) ist ein US amerikanischer Linguist. Der Schwerpunkt seiner Arbeit ist die Klassifikation von Sprachen weltweit und die Evolution des Menschen im Lichte seiner Sprachfähigkeit. Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Laufbahn 2… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Merritt Ruhlen — Nom de naissance Merritt Ruhlen Naissance 10 mai 1944 (67 ans) Nationalité  États Unis …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Merritt Ruhlen — (nacido el 10 de mayo de 1944 en Washington D.C.) es un lingüista norteamericano. Su trabajo más conocido está dedicado a la clasificación de las lenguas del mundo[1] y a la evolución de la humanidad desde la perspectiva de la actividad… …   Wikipedia Español

  • Ruhlen — Merritt Ruhlen (* 10. Mai 1944 in Washington, D.C.) ist ein US amerikanischer Linguist. Der Schwerpunkt seiner Arbeit ist die Klassifikation von Sprachen weltweit und die Evolution des Menschen im Lichte seiner Sprachfähigkeit. Inhaltsverzeichnis …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Меррит Рулен — Меррит Рулен, англ. Merritt Ruhlen /ˈmɛɹɪt ˈɹuːlən/ (род. 1944[1]) американский лингвист, известен своими работами по классификации языков, палеолингвистике и эволюции языков. Взгляды Рулена испытали сильное влияние Дж. Гринберга, с которым он… …   Википедия

  • Рулен Меррит — Меррит Рулен, англ. Merritt Ruhlen /ˈmɛɹɪt ˈɹuːlən/ (род. 1944[1]) американский лингвист, известен своими работами по классификации языков, палеолингвистике и эволюции языков. Взгляды Рулена испытали сильное влияние Дж. Гринберга, с которым он… …   Википедия

  • Amerind languages — Amerind is a putative higher level language family proposed by Joseph Greenberg in his 1987 book Language in the Americas . In this book Greenberg proposed that all of the indigenous languages of the Americas belong to one of three families.… …   Wikipedia

  • Рулен, Меррит — Меррит Рулен (англ. Merritt Ruhlen /ˈmɛɹɪt ˈɹuːlən/, род. 1944[1])  американский лингвист, известен своими работами по классификации языков, палеолингвистике и эволюции языков. Взгляды Рулена испытали сильное влияние Дж. Гринберга, с… …   Википедия

  • Рулен — Рулен, Меррит Меррит Рулен, англ. Merritt Ruhlen /ˈmɛɹɪt ˈɹuːlən/ (род. 1944[1]) американский лингвист, известен своими работами по классификации языков, палеолингвистике и эволюции языков. Взгляды Рулена испытали сильное влияние Дж. Гринберга, с …   Википедия

  • Proto-Human language — The Proto Human language (also Proto Sapiens, Proto World) is the hypothetical most recent common ancestor of all the world s languages. The concept of Proto Human presupposes monogenesis of all recorded spoken human languages. It does not… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”