Müller-Lyer illusion

Müller-Lyer illusion
Two sets of arrows that exhibit the Müller-Lyer optical illusion. The set on the bottom shows that all the arrows are of the same length.

The Müller-Lyer illusion is an optical illusion consisting of a stylized arrow. When viewers are asked to place a mark on the figure at the mid-point, they invariably place it more towards the "tail" end. It was devised by F.C. Müller-Lyer in 1889.[1][2][3]

A variation of the same illusion (and the most common form in which it is seen today, see figure) consists of a set of arrow-like figures. Straight line segments comprise the "shafts" of the arrows, while shorter line segments (called the fins) protrude from the ends of the shaft. The fins can point inwards to form an arrow "head" or outwards to form an arrow "tail". The line segment forming the shaft of the arrow with two tails is perceived to be longer than that forming the shaft of the arrow with two heads.

Contents

Variation in Perception

It has been shown that perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion varies across cultures and age groups.

Segall, Campbell and Herskovitz[4] compared susceptibility to four different visual illusions in three population samples of Caucasians, twelve of Africans, and one from the Philippines. For the Müller-Lyer illusion, the mean fractional misperception of the length of the line segments varied from 1.4% to 20.3%. The three European-derived samples were the three most susceptible samples.

It has been suggested that the cross-cultural variation in the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion is due to differences in the familiar objects of the subjects' environments. In Western cultures full of rectilinear shapes (the so-called "carpentered world"), subjects perceive the lines as representing the corners of rectilinear objects viewed in perspective.

A later study was conducted by Ahluwalia[5] on children and young adults from Zambia. Subjects from rural areas were compared with subjects from urban areas. The subjects from urban areas were shown to be considerably more susceptible to the illusion, as were younger subjects. While this by no means confirms the carpentered world hypothesis as such, it provides evidence that differences in the environment can create differences in the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion, even within a given culture.

Experiments have been reported[6] suggesting that pigeons perceive the standard Müller-Lyer illusion, but not the reversed. Experiments on parrots have also been reported with similar results.[7]

The perspective explanation

The Müller-Lyer effect in a non-illusion

One possible explanation, given by Richard Gregory,[8] states that the Müller-Lyer illusion occurs because the visual system processes that judge depth and distance assume in general that the “angles in” configuration corresponds to an object which is closer, and the “angles out” configuration corresponds to an object which is far away. Basically, there seems to be a simple heuristic that takes those configurations as 90° angles. This heuristic speeds up the interpretation process, but gives rise to many optical illusions in unusual scenes. A recent report[9] by Catherine Howe and Dale Purves summarizes current thinking on Gregory's ideas:

Although Gregory's intuition about the empirical significance of the Müller-Lyer stimulus points in the right general direction (i.e., an explanation based on past experience with the sources of such stimuli), convex and concave corners contribute little if anything to the Müller-Lyer effect.

Neural nets in the visual system of human beings learn how to make a very efficient interpretation of 3D scenes. That is why when somebody goes away from us, we do not perceive him as getting shorter. And when we stretch one arm and look at the two hands we do not perceive one hand smaller than the other. We should not forget that, as visual illusions show us quite clearly, what we see is an image created in our brain. Our brain projects the image of the smaller hand to its correct distance in our internal 3D model. This is what is called the size constancy mechanism.

In the Müller-Lyer illusion, the visual system would in this explanation detect the depth cues, which are usually associated with 3D scenes, and incorrectly decide it is a 3D drawing. Then the size constancy mechanism would make us see an erroneous length of the object which, for a true perspective drawing, would be farther away.

In the perspective drawing in the figure, we see that in usual scenes the heuristic works quite well. The width of the rug should obviously be considered shorter than the length of the wall in the back.

References

  1. ^ Müller-Lyer, FC (1889), "Optische Urteilstäuschungen"; Archiv für Physiologie Suppl. 263–270.
  2. ^ Brentano, F (1892), "Über ein optisches Paradoxen", Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 3:349–358.
  3. ^ Müller-Lyer, FC (1894), "Über Kontrast und Konfluxion", Zeitschrift für Psychologie, IX p 1 / X p 421.
  4. ^ Cultural Differences in the Perception of Geometric Illusions Author(s): Marshall H. Segall, Donald T. Campbell, Melville J. Herskovits Source: Science, New Series, Vol. 139, No. 3556 (February 22, 1963), pp. 769-771
  5. ^ An intra-cultural investigation of susceptibility to 'perspective' and 'non-perspective' spatial illusions, Br. J. of Psychol.,1978,69,233-241
  6. ^ Nakamura et al., Noriyuki. "Perception of the Standard and the Reversed Müller-Lyer Figures in Pigeons (Columba livia) and Humans (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2006 August Vol 120(3) 252-261. http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=main.doiLanding&uid=2006-09888-010. Retrieved 2008-07-25. 
  7. ^ Pepperberg et al., Irene. "The Müller-Lyer illusion is processed by a Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus)". Perception 37:765-781. http://www.alexfoundation.org/pdf/irene_pdf/PerceptionAlexML.pdf. Retrieved 2011-07-30. 
  8. ^ Richard L. Gregory, Eye and Brain, McGraw Hill, 1966.
  9. ^ The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image–source relationships Catherine Q. Howe and Dale Purves* PNAS January 25, 2005 vol. 102 no. 4 1234-1239

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Müller-Lyer illusion —    Also known as arrowhead illusion. The eponym Müller Lyer illusion refers to the German sociologist Franz Carl Müller Lyer (1857 1916), who described the concomitant phenomenon in or shortly before 1889. This phenomenon consists of a *… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • Müller-Lyer-Illusion — Die obere waagerechte Linie erscheint kürzer als die untere Die Müller Lyer Illusion, auch Müller Lyer Täuschung, ist vermutlich die bekannteste geometrisch optische Täuschung. Sie wurde 1889 von dem deutschen Psychiater und Soziologen Franz… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Müller-Lyer illusion — /mul euhr luy euhr, myooh leuhr , mil euhr /; Ger. /myuul euhrdd lee euhrdd/ a geometric illusion in which two lines of equal length appear unequal depending on whether angular lines forming arrowheads at each end point toward or away from each… …   Universalium

  • Müller-Lyer illusion — /mul euhr luy euhr, myooh leuhr , mil euhr /; Ger. /myuul euhrdd lee euhrdd/ a geometric illusion in which two lines of equal length appear unequal depending on whether angular lines forming arrowheads at each end point toward or away from each… …   Useful english dictionary

  • Müller-Lyer illusion — noun An optical illusion consisting of a stylized arrow whose midpoint appears to be closer to the tail end …   Wiktionary

  • Müller–Lyer illusion — The visual illusion in which outward pointing arrowheads at the end of a line increase its apparent length, and inward pointing ones decrease it, compared to a neutral line of the same length …   Philosophy dictionary

  • Illusion De Müller-Lyer — Les deux groupes de flèches sont exactement les mêmes. Le deuxième prouve que les flèches ont exactement la même longueur. L Illusion de Müller Lyer est une illusion d optique perceptive distordante célèbre qui se résume à une flèche. Quand on… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Illusion de Muller-Lyer — Illusion de Müller Lyer Les deux groupes de flèches sont exactement les mêmes. Le deuxième prouve que les flèches ont exactement la même longueur. L Illusion de Müller Lyer est une illusion d optique perceptive distordante célèbre qui se résume à …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Illusion de müller-lyer — Les deux groupes de flèches sont exactement les mêmes. Le deuxième prouve que les flèches ont exactement la même longueur. L Illusion de Müller Lyer est une illusion d optique perceptive distordante célèbre qui se résume à une flèche. Quand on… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Ilusión de Müller-Lyer — La Ilusión de Müller Lyer, descubierta por F. C. Muller Lyer en 1889, es una ilusión óptica en la que dos o más segmentos de igual tamaño parecen más grandes o más pequeños dependiendo de que las puntas de flecha añadidos en sus extremos apunten… …   Wikipedia Español

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”